2015
DOI: 10.1111/bjdp.12121
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Detection of counting pseudoerrors: What helps children accept them?

Abstract: This study examines children's comprehension of non-essential counting features (conventional rules). The objective of the study was to determine whether the presence or absence of cardinal values in pseudoerrors and the type of conventional rule violated affects children's performance. A detection task with pseudoerrors was presented through a computer game to 146 primary school children in grades 2 through 4. The same pseudoerrors were presented both with and without cardinal values; the pseudoerrors violate… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

4
6
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
3

Relationship

2
1

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 3 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 26 publications
4
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The mean percentage of rejected pseudoerrors before listening to the testimony reached 70.4% of trials, which is comparable to the 73.7% obtained by Rodríguez, Lago, Enesco, and Guerrero (2013) with children of similar ages. Furthermore, the results are consistent with those obtained in other studies regarding the justifications provided by the children when rejecting the pseudoerrors (e.g., Lago et al, 2016; Rodríguez et al, 2013). They also confirm that understanding of the optional nature of the conventional rules of counting continues to develop beyond the early years of primary education, as suggested by several authors (e.g., Escudero et al, 2015; LeFevre et al, 2006; Kamawar et al, 2010).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…The mean percentage of rejected pseudoerrors before listening to the testimony reached 70.4% of trials, which is comparable to the 73.7% obtained by Rodríguez, Lago, Enesco, and Guerrero (2013) with children of similar ages. Furthermore, the results are consistent with those obtained in other studies regarding the justifications provided by the children when rejecting the pseudoerrors (e.g., Lago et al, 2016; Rodríguez et al, 2013). They also confirm that understanding of the optional nature of the conventional rules of counting continues to develop beyond the early years of primary education, as suggested by several authors (e.g., Escudero et al, 2015; LeFevre et al, 2006; Kamawar et al, 2010).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
“…In overall terms, our findings lend support to those of other studies suggesting that in certain conditions children may be less inflexible when judging unconventional counts (Escudero et al, 2015; Lago et al, 2016; Rodríguez et al, 2013). Our findings also provide further evidence in support of the idea that trust in the testimony of others is key as a source of learning and this trust is enhanced when it is placed in a unanimous majority (Bernard et al, 2015; Corriveau et al, 2009; Morgan et al, 2015).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 90%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Empirical evidence with the detection paradigm has demonstrated that children have misconceptions about the non-essential nature of conventional counting rules even well into primary school (LeFevre et al, 2006;Kamawar et al, 2010;Rodríguez et al, 2013;Escudero et al, 2015;Lago et al, 2016Lago et al, , 2019. In these studies, children observed a character performing standard correct counts (that comply both with logical and conventional rules), erroneous counts (which break logical rules), and pseudoerrors (not conventional but correct counts that respect logical rules) and were asked to evaluate the correctness of their performance.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%