1987
DOI: 10.1016/0042-6989(87)90174-x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Detection threshold differences to crossed and uncrossed disparities

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

5
29
2

Year Published

1989
1989
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 61 publications
(36 citation statements)
references
References 8 publications
5
29
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Patterson, et al, 1995) and spatial (e.g. Manning, et al, 1987) limits, tolerances for polarity reversal , and rates of development (Birch, Gwiazda, & Held, 1982), for various such mechanisms. However, such findings have also been interpreted as consistent with a model that does not posit distinct disparity pooling mechanisms (Landers & Cormack, 1997).…”
Section: Functional Organizationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Patterson, et al, 1995) and spatial (e.g. Manning, et al, 1987) limits, tolerances for polarity reversal , and rates of development (Birch, Gwiazda, & Held, 1982), for various such mechanisms. However, such findings have also been interpreted as consistent with a model that does not posit distinct disparity pooling mechanisms (Landers & Cormack, 1997).…”
Section: Functional Organizationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As pointed out by Manning et al (1987), there may be inherent differences between detecting a shape in front ofa background plane versus detecting a plane behind an aperture of some shape. Other research does suggest that disparity interpolation is more efficient for surfaces of crossed disparity than for those of uncrossed disparity (Yang & Blake, 1995), attesting to the complex nature of such stimuli.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Manning, Finlay, Neill, and Frost (1987) measured stimulus duration thresholds for crossed and uncrossed disparities at various positions in the visual field. At every position they tested, longer durations were required to detect uncrossed disparities than crossed disparities.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In line with Previc's theoretical model, most investigators have shown that crossed-disparity (i.e., near) stimuli are better processed in the LVF, whereas uncrossed-disparity (i.e., far) stimuli are detected more readily in the UVF (Breitmeyer, Julesz & Kropfi, 1975;Breitmeyer, Weinstein & Previc, 1992;Julesz, Breitmeyer & Kropfi, 1976; but see Manning, Finlay, Neill & Frost, 1987). This trend may also interact with an overall advantage for near stimuli (Grabowska, 1983;Harwerth & Boltz, 1979;Lasley, Ktvlin, Rich & Flynn, 1984;Mustillo, 1985) and left-right hemifield differences in global depth perception (Breitmeyer et al, 1992;Durnford & Kimura, 1971;Grabowska, 1983;Manning et al, 1987).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 88%
“…This trend may also interact with an overall advantage for near stimuli (Grabowska, 1983;Harwerth & Boltz, 1979;Lasley, Ktvlin, Rich & Flynn, 1984;Mustillo, 1985) and left-right hemifield differences in global depth perception (Breitmeyer et al, 1992;Durnford & Kimura, 1971;Grabowska, 1983;Manning et al, 1987).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%