Handbook of Emotional Development 2019
DOI: 10.1007/978-3-030-17332-6_12
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Developing Disgust: Theory, Measurement, and Application

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
13
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

2
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 17 publications
(13 citation statements)
references
References 224 publications
0
13
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In Study 1, a group of adults was surveyed about the items they and their children find disgusting. As we noted above, prior measures of disgust sensitivity for children were adapted from adult measures (Muris et al, 2012; Nadeau et al, 2017; Schienle & Schwab, 2019; Viar-Paxton et al, 2015), and theories of a mature theory of disgust focus on abstract concepts, rather than sensory properties (Rozin & Fallon, 1987; for a review, see Rottman et al, 2019). As such, these measures tend to omit questions about uncontaminated foods when examining disgust sensitivity.…”
Section: Study 1: Online Parent Surveymentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In Study 1, a group of adults was surveyed about the items they and their children find disgusting. As we noted above, prior measures of disgust sensitivity for children were adapted from adult measures (Muris et al, 2012; Nadeau et al, 2017; Schienle & Schwab, 2019; Viar-Paxton et al, 2015), and theories of a mature theory of disgust focus on abstract concepts, rather than sensory properties (Rozin & Fallon, 1987; for a review, see Rottman et al, 2019). As such, these measures tend to omit questions about uncontaminated foods when examining disgust sensitivity.…”
Section: Study 1: Online Parent Surveymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, rather than eliciting responses from children regarding what they find to be disgusting, disgust sensitivity scales for children have typically been directly adapted from existing adult scales (Muris et al, 2012; Nadeau et al, 2017; Schienle & Schwab, 2019; Viar-Paxton et al, 2015), but without items that might be inappropriate for young children (e.g., those dealing with sexual behavior). In addition, theories of the development of disgust have often focused on the transition from concrete to abstract thinking (for a review, see Rottman et al, 2019). Abstract, conceptual disgust elicitors (which are viewed as a more mature form of disgust) are difficult for younger children to understand (especially those younger than age 6), as these children are thought to lack the cognitive prerequisites to consider invisible entities, such as germs or other contaminants (Rozin & Fallon, 1987).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Sensitivity to different types of disgust-inducing threats also emerges over early childhood, and is largely absent when infants and toddlers are most vulnerable to infection and disease. Feeling disgusted by other people (as opposed to bodily fluids or animals) appears particularly late, around 7 years of age [45].…”
Section: Origin Storiesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Preschoolers do not differentiate between eating a clean versus germ-contaminated food (DeJesus, Shutts, & Kinzler, 2015) and do not avoid contact with someone who is "sick" (Blacker & LoBue, 2016). In fact, it is not until age 5 or 6 that these capacities reliably emerge (for a review, see Rottman, DeJesus, & Greenebaum, 2019). It is important to note that some conceptual causal knowledge of germ contagion is relatively early-emerging (Blacker & LoBue, 2016;Raman & Gelman, 2008).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%