2010
DOI: 10.1016/j.wocn.2010.06.002
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Development of voicing perception in French: Comparing adults, adolescents, and children

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

4
40
0
21

Year Published

2011
2011
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

3
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 46 publications
(65 citation statements)
references
References 48 publications
4
40
0
21
Order By: Relevance
“…At the nonspeech level, this is in line with (1) physiological animal studies showing that neural coding for temporal aspects of the stimulus reaches maturity later than neural coding for frequency selectivity (Eggermont, 1996), and with (2) behavioral auditory studies in humans providing evidence for a more prolonged development of the sensitivity for temporal than for nontemporal auditory cues (Hartley, Wright, Hogan, & Moore, 2000), even after accounting for the effect of procedure-related skills (Dawes & Bishop, 2008). At the speech level, it coincides with behavioral speech perception studies demonstrating that the identification of stop consonants is not yet mature by the age of 11 (Hazan & Barrett, 2000;Johnson, 2000;Krause, 1982;Simon & Fourcin, 1978;Medina, Hoonhorst, Bogliotti, & Serniclaes, 2010), whereas the identification of vowels does only slightly, though not significantly, improves towards adolescence (Pursell, Swanson, Hedrick, & Nabelek, 2002;Ohde, Haley, & McMahon, 1996;Johnson, 2000, but see Walley and Flege, 1999). Further elaboration on this topic is needed, but the indication that perception of sounds with temporal versus nontemporal cues follows different maturational trajectories in both normal and dyslexic readers, may have practical implications with regard to auditory temporal training programs.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 61%
“…At the nonspeech level, this is in line with (1) physiological animal studies showing that neural coding for temporal aspects of the stimulus reaches maturity later than neural coding for frequency selectivity (Eggermont, 1996), and with (2) behavioral auditory studies in humans providing evidence for a more prolonged development of the sensitivity for temporal than for nontemporal auditory cues (Hartley, Wright, Hogan, & Moore, 2000), even after accounting for the effect of procedure-related skills (Dawes & Bishop, 2008). At the speech level, it coincides with behavioral speech perception studies demonstrating that the identification of stop consonants is not yet mature by the age of 11 (Hazan & Barrett, 2000;Johnson, 2000;Krause, 1982;Simon & Fourcin, 1978;Medina, Hoonhorst, Bogliotti, & Serniclaes, 2010), whereas the identification of vowels does only slightly, though not significantly, improves towards adolescence (Pursell, Swanson, Hedrick, & Nabelek, 2002;Ohde, Haley, & McMahon, 1996;Johnson, 2000, but see Walley and Flege, 1999). Further elaboration on this topic is needed, but the indication that perception of sounds with temporal versus nontemporal cues follows different maturational trajectories in both normal and dyslexic readers, may have practical implications with regard to auditory temporal training programs.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 61%
“…Children have also been found to be incapable of using the fine-grained spectral cues under adult-directed speech as well as adults do in discrimination and identification tasks [9][10]. Nevertheless, other studies have failed to observe age variations in identification tasks among children from grade 2 to grade 5 and in speech discrimination tasks among school-aged children, teenagers, and adults [11][12]. While most studies were conducted in children with non-tonal languages, only limited studies investigated speech perception in young children with tonal languages such as Mandarin Chinese and Cantonese [13]–[15].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The children enrolled in this study received their Cl between 1.5 and 3.5 years, and as phonemic categories develop from birth in typically developing children (Hoonhorst et al, 2009), their central auditory system may already have reorganised in a way that limits an optimal benefit from the implant (Krai & Sharma, 2012;Sharma, Dorman, & Krai, 2005). Alter natively, it might be that the early auditory deficit cannot fully be compensated for within the timeframe during which the phonological system remains plastic (Medina et al, 2010). Overall, poorer phonological processing in children who use a Cl presumably reflects a combination of hearing deprivation and degraded auditory input (Dorman et al, 2002;Shannon et al, 1995;Xu & Zheng, 2007), and this might place some constrains on the way the children learn to read (Kraus & Anderson, 2013).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The acoustic-to-linguistic mapping hence implies that speech sounds that can perceptually be discriminated may be assigned to the same or to different speech phonemic representational category/ies, which are subsequently used for phoneme/ grapheme mapping when learning to read. In TH children, phonemic categories emerge early in life (Burnham, Eamshaw, & Clark, 1991;Hazan & Barrett, 2000;Hoonhorst et al, 2011) and continue refining during the first six years of life (Bernstein, 1983;Hazan & Barrett, 2000;Nittroucr, 1992Nittroucr, , 1996Nittroucr, , 2002Nittrouer & Miller, 1997;Slawinski & Fitzgerald, 1998), with flexible boundaries up to the beginning of adolescence (Burnham et al, 1991;Hazan & Barrett, 2000;Medina, Hoonhorst, Bogliotti, & Semiclaes, 2010).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%