Handbook of Juvenile Forensic Psychology and Psychiatry 2012
DOI: 10.1007/978-1-4614-0905-2_2
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Developmental Changes in Adolescence and Risks for Delinquency

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

1
34
0
1

Year Published

2015
2015
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 28 publications
(36 citation statements)
references
References 98 publications
1
34
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…They interpreted this result as a compensating effect, in which the “wave” of DT might be in a “slump” when the wave of reasoning is at its “peak” (see also Guignard & Lubart, in press). Many similar examples can be found in the neurodevelopmental literature, highlighting, for example, a temporal gap between the development of the socioemotional system and the cognitive control systems in adolescence, leading to maladaptive behavior such as risk taking and impulsivity (e.g., Barbot & Hunter, ), which could also have implications for the development of person‐level resources important for creativity.…”
Section: Foundations Of Individual Differences In Creativity Developmentmentioning
confidence: 73%
“…They interpreted this result as a compensating effect, in which the “wave” of DT might be in a “slump” when the wave of reasoning is at its “peak” (see also Guignard & Lubart, in press). Many similar examples can be found in the neurodevelopmental literature, highlighting, for example, a temporal gap between the development of the socioemotional system and the cognitive control systems in adolescence, leading to maladaptive behavior such as risk taking and impulsivity (e.g., Barbot & Hunter, ), which could also have implications for the development of person‐level resources important for creativity.…”
Section: Foundations Of Individual Differences In Creativity Developmentmentioning
confidence: 73%
“…On a related note, recent advances of research into the neurobiological development of the brain suggest that at least some of the constituent components of self‐control are likely to develop more in accordance with the age–crime curve than others and might therefore be conceptually closer to the vividness of the future self concept. Specifically, this research has shown that risk seeking and impulsivity, which are conflated in most existing measures of self‐control (Burt, Sweeten, and Simons, ), develop along different maturational timetables (Barbot and Hunter, ; Casey, Jones, and Hare, ; Steinberg, ). As Steinberg et al (: 39) remarked, researchers “interested in the course of ‘future orientation’ should bear in mind that different aspects of future orientation may follow different developmental trajectories and reach adult levels of maturity at different ages.” These findings tie in nicely with the vividness concept as they imply that the ability of people to consider the future is likely to develop with age.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In contrast, similar to future orientation, i.e., the capacity and inclination to project events into the future (Steinberg, ), we expect that vividness of the future self increases with age; being weak during adolescence and developing considerably toward early adulthood. As Barbot and Hunter (: 22–3) remarked, adolescents' relatively limited life experience means that they perceive future time differently from adults in the sense that they are less able to perceive the proximity of the future and are consequently less inclined to heavily weigh future consequences. To an adolescent, short‐term consequences are therefore likely to have far greater salience than consequences located several years away in the future, whereas long‐term consequences of present‐day decisions are likely to seem more immediate with increasing age (Barbot and Hunter, ; Steinberg, ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations