2012
DOI: 10.1007/s11055-012-9572-4
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Developmental Changes in Measures of Hierarchical Stimulus Recognition in Conditions of Directed Attention in Children Aged 5–10 Years

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

1
4
0

Year Published

2013
2013
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
5

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 17 publications
1
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…First, our results showed that local targets were perceptively less salient from 5-year-old children to adulthood, with slower RTs compared with other levels and more errors for the two younger age groups (5-and 6-year-old children). We thus replicated the results of previous studies using hierarchical figures with normaldensity (Dukette & Stiles, 1996;Harrison & Stiles, 2009;Kimchi et al, 2005;Krupskaya & Machinskaya, 2012;Mondloch et al, 2003;Ozonoff et al, 1994;Plaisted et al, 1999). Our results also reported an age-variability for the most salient level in a competitive context (when the target simultaneously appears with distractors).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…First, our results showed that local targets were perceptively less salient from 5-year-old children to adulthood, with slower RTs compared with other levels and more errors for the two younger age groups (5-and 6-year-old children). We thus replicated the results of previous studies using hierarchical figures with normaldensity (Dukette & Stiles, 1996;Harrison & Stiles, 2009;Kimchi et al, 2005;Krupskaya & Machinskaya, 2012;Mondloch et al, 2003;Ozonoff et al, 1994;Plaisted et al, 1999). Our results also reported an age-variability for the most salient level in a competitive context (when the target simultaneously appears with distractors).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
“…Nevertheless, results appear to diverge for older children, depending on the task used; the size, density and meaning of the hierarchical stimuli; and the condition of occurrence (Dukette & Stiles, 1996; Poirel, Mellet, et al, 2008). In classical Navon-type detection tasks, a global bias is reported in 5-to-6-year-old (Krupskaya & Machinskaya, 2012; Ozonoff, Strayer, McMahon, & Filloux, 1994; Plaisted, Swettenham, & Rees, 1999). Nevertheless, using a different paradigm with smaller figures, Scherf, Behrmann, Kimchi, and Luna (2009) observed the existence of a local bias that disappeared only at the end of adolescence.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…In particular, global features capture their perception in selective attention tasks. While both adults and children can attend to global and local aspects of stimuli, there are many contexts where young children prioritize global features (Huizinga, Burack, & Van der Molen, 2010; Kimchi, Hadad, Behrmann, & Palmer, 2005; Krupskaya & Machinskaya, 2012; Vinter, Puspitawati, & Witt, 2010). Children can use overall similarity between multidimensional stimuli as a main categorization strategy (Smith, 1981), for example, grouping cartoon people by number of shared features, despite knowledge of the critical category feature (a symbol on their stomach; Deng & Sloutsky, 2015).…”
Section: Selective Attention To Perceptual Features Across Developmentmentioning
confidence: 99%