1980
DOI: 10.1037/0012-1649.16.4.312
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Developmental changes in the effects of landmarks on infant spatial behavior.

Abstract: The ability of 6-, 9-, and 11-month-olds to keep track of positions in space was assessed under four conditions varying the presence, salience, and location of landmark information. Each infant was trained to expect an event at one of two windows to either side. The infants were than rotated to the opposite side of the room, and the direction of search was interpreted as an indication of whether the event's location was being coded egocentrically or objectively. The results indicated high proportions of egocen… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

8
144
2

Year Published

1984
1984
2013
2013

Publication Types

Select...
5
4

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 196 publications
(154 citation statements)
references
References 8 publications
8
144
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Explorations of the mechanisms that support it have provided insight into, for example, the development of spatial orientation in infancy (e.g. Bremner, Hatton, Foster, & Mason, 2011;Acredolo & Evans, 1980) and this literature has been complemented by very useful reviews of the relationship between vision, locomotion, and spatial orientation in childhood, as provided by Campos et al (2000) and Rieser & Pick (2007). More recently, a study by Nardini, Jones, Bedford, & Braddick (2008) has explored how information from path integration is combined with visual cues in order to support place memory.…”
Section: Development Of Path Integrationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Explorations of the mechanisms that support it have provided insight into, for example, the development of spatial orientation in infancy (e.g. Bremner, Hatton, Foster, & Mason, 2011;Acredolo & Evans, 1980) and this literature has been complemented by very useful reviews of the relationship between vision, locomotion, and spatial orientation in childhood, as provided by Campos et al (2000) and Rieser & Pick (2007). More recently, a study by Nardini, Jones, Bedford, & Braddick (2008) has explored how information from path integration is combined with visual cues in order to support place memory.…”
Section: Development Of Path Integrationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Acredolo (1978Acredolo ( , 1979Acredolo & Evans, 1980), in particular, examined how children use egocentric and environmental reference systems. In a series of studies, she trained children to make a particular response relative to both an egocentric and an environmental reference system.…”
Section: Environmental Reference Systemsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Acredolo (1978) found that very young children made egocentric responses, but between 6 and 16 months of age children began using the environmental information, turning to the landmark (window). Additional experiments suggested that the use of such information was facilitated when landmarks were made more salient (Acredolo & Evans, 1980) or when children were placed in familiar environments (Acredolo, 1979).…”
Section: Environmental Reference Systemsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The earliest form of externally referenced location coding is cue learning (e.g., the color or pattern of the screen that occluded the object); this is effective enough for 9-month-olds to search correctly when they have been moved after watching an object hidden under distinctive screens (Bremner, 1978b). With development, spatial coding begins to take into account landmarks in the environment (Acredolo & Evans, 1980;DeLoache & Brown, 1983), that is, perceptually distinctive, fixed elements of the locale. Because an object's location can now be coded relative to landmarks, this affords accurate search after movement even when the object is hidden by identical occluders or in an unmarked location (Newcombe, Huttenlocher, Drummey, & Wiley, 1998).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%