1982
DOI: 10.1016/0022-0965(82)90021-2
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Developmental effects of typicality and superordinate property dominance on sentence verification

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
9
0

Year Published

1983
1983
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 11 publications
1
9
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The internal structure of a semantic category includes a prototype at the core, which is the most central item representing the category. The prototype itself may be an idealized or abstract representation of the category (e.g., dog is a more prototypical or ideal representation than bat for the category "animals"; Keller, 1982;Rosch, 1973). Category members vary with respect to the number of attributes they share with the prototype (Rosch & Mervis, 1975).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The internal structure of a semantic category includes a prototype at the core, which is the most central item representing the category. The prototype itself may be an idealized or abstract representation of the category (e.g., dog is a more prototypical or ideal representation than bat for the category "animals"; Keller, 1982;Rosch, 1973). Category members vary with respect to the number of attributes they share with the prototype (Rosch & Mervis, 1975).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Age-related improvement might be related to increasing, and more robust, conceptual knowledge, particularly regarding the properties characterizing clothing and their relative importance (Duncan & Kellas, 1978;Keller, 1982;Schwanenflugel, Guth, & Bjorkland, 1986;Younger & Mekos, 1992). Age-related improvement might also be related to increased processing proficiency in selective attention and inhibition (Bjorklund, 2005).…”
Section: Do Variations In Typicality and Relatednessmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…In children, for example, typical items are classified as members of a category developmentally earlier than atypical items (Mervis, 1987;Whitney & Kunen, 1980). Developmental changes in category membership may be observed up to the preteen years and may reflect children's increasing 1) specification of the properties characterizing a category; 2) appreciation of the statistical regularities of, and intercorrelations among, properties; and 3) realization that some properties are more important to membership than others (Duncan & Kellas, 1978;Keller, 1982;Schwanenflugel, Guth, & Bjorklund, 1986;Younger & Mekos, 1992).…”
Section: Typicality Effectmentioning
confidence: 97%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In children, typical items are classified as members of a category developmentally earlier than are atypical items (Mervis, 1989;Whitney & Kunen, 1983). Developmental changes in category membership may again be observed up to the preadolescent years and may reflect childrenÕs increasing (a) specification of the properties characterizing a category; (b) appreciation of the statistical regularities of, and intercorrelations among, properties; and (c) realization that some properties are more important to membership than are others (Duncan & Kellas, 1978;Keller, 1982;Schwanenflugel, Guth, & Bjorklund, 1986;Younger & Mekos, 1992).…”
Section: Typicality Effectmentioning
confidence: 96%