Canadian students in grades 5 and 6 studied facts about the 10 Canadian provinces (e.g. Many very good plays are put on in Prince Edward Island). At study, each fact was accompanied by a picture of a stereotypical setting in the province (e.g. a picture of the lush farmland in Prince Edward Island). Half the subjects were instructed to learn by imagining the fact as stated occurring in the depiction (e.g. very good plays being produced in the lush farmland setting); half were told only to try hard to remember that the fact occurred in the particular province. The imagery instruction was effective, but only when children had previously mastered associations between the provinces and the pictures of the provinces. Naturalisticaliy occurring pictures can be used by grade-school children to create elaborative images that facilitate acquisition of confusing facts, but only if children first know what the illustrations represent.Children are often called upon to learn factual information, including scientific facts (e.g. characteristics of chemical elements), biographical content, and information about particular places. These tasks can be difficult even for proficient adult learners (e.g. Bransford, Stein, Vye, Franks, Auble, Mezynski and Perfetto, 1982;Pressley, Symons, McDaniel, Snyder and Turnure, 1988), let alone children, who were studied in the investigation reported here. Although we do not agree with extreme arguments that even more fact learning should be presented in school (e.g. Hirsch, 1987;Ravitch and Finn, 1987), the frequency of fact learning, and the challenges it poses, combine to fuel the search for mechanisms that promote learning of facts. Our position is that if children are going to be required to learn facts, they should be armed with procedures that permit efficient acquisition of factual content. To date, most procedures that have been studied are various forms of elaboration (Pressley, 1982;Pressley, Johnson and Symons, 1987).For instance, Bransford and his associates demonstrated that children experience difficulty when attempting to learn sentences containing mutually interfering pieces of information, such as the following:The hungry man got into the car. The strong man helped the woman. The brave man ran into the house.When children are taught to elaborate these sentences so that it makes clear why each particular man performed the action that he performed (e.g. The hungry man got into the car to go to the restaurant; The brave man ran into the house that was on 088&4080/90/050359-11$05.50