1987
DOI: 10.3758/bf03205021
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Differential acquisition of discriminated autoshaping as a function of stimulus qualities and locations

Abstract: In each of two experiments, different groups of pigeons were required to discriminate between one of two basic kinds of stimulus differences: stimulus quality or stimulus location. For stimulusquality groups, a key was illuminated by one of two colors on trials ending with food delivery and by the other color on trials ending with no food. For stimulus-location groups, a key was illuminated at one of two locations on trials ending with food delivery and at the other location on trials ending with no food. The … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

0
3
0

Year Published

1992
1992
2003
2003

Publication Types

Select...
3

Relationship

0
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 3 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 7 publications
0
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Changes in response probabilities with changes in trial intervals and ITIs (Balsam & Tomie, 1985;Gibbon & Balsam, 1981;Jenkins, Barnes, & Barrera, 1981) may be associated with changes in those parameters. Pretraining (Killeen, 1984;Linden, Savage, & Overmier, 1997) may play a crucial role by establishing initial values for p. Various other controlling variables in autoshaped performance (e.g., Bowe, Green, & Miller, 1987;Ploog & Zeigler, 1996;Silva, Silva, & Pear, 1992;Steinhauer, 1982) may effect their control through changing system parameters such as those modeled in Figure 4, or in more complicated neural network models (e.g., J. W. Moore & Stickney, 1982). Figure 6 displayed the close relation between the expected probability of a response and the rate of responding on the subsequent trial.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Changes in response probabilities with changes in trial intervals and ITIs (Balsam & Tomie, 1985;Gibbon & Balsam, 1981;Jenkins, Barnes, & Barrera, 1981) may be associated with changes in those parameters. Pretraining (Killeen, 1984;Linden, Savage, & Overmier, 1997) may play a crucial role by establishing initial values for p. Various other controlling variables in autoshaped performance (e.g., Bowe, Green, & Miller, 1987;Ploog & Zeigler, 1996;Silva, Silva, & Pear, 1992;Steinhauer, 1982) may effect their control through changing system parameters such as those modeled in Figure 4, or in more complicated neural network models (e.g., J. W. Moore & Stickney, 1982). Figure 6 displayed the close relation between the expected probability of a response and the rate of responding on the subsequent trial.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Pretraining (Killeen, 1984; Linden, Savage, & Overmier, 1997) may play a crucial role by establishing initial values for p . Various other controlling variables in autoshaped performance (e.g., Bowe, Green, & Miller, 1987; Ploog & Zeigler, 1996; Silva, Silva, & Pear, 1992; Steinhauer, 1982) may effect their control through changing system parameters such as those modeled in Figure 4, or in more complicated neural network models (e.g., J. W. Moore & Stickney, 1982).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…So, too, does using CSs and USs with similar attributes (Testa 1975; see also Rescorla & Furrow 1977, Rescorla & Gillan 1980. Thorndikean discrimination learning is aided by the instrumental response spatially coinciding with the discriminative stimulus (Harrison et al 1977, Ramey & Goulet 1971cf Rumbaugh et al 1989) and by the discriminative and reinforcing stimuli sharing similar patterns of stimulation (for a review, see JD Miller & Bowe 1982; for data, see Bowe et al 1987;cf Neill & Harrison 1987).…”
Section: Simple Acquisition With a Single Cue Or Responsementioning
confidence: 99%