2015
DOI: 10.1177/0739986315571113
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Differing Cognitive Trajectories of Mexican American Toddlers

Abstract: Recent studies reveal early and wide gaps in cognitive and oral language skills-whether gauged in English or Spanish-among Latino children relative to White peers. Yet, other work reports robust child health and social development, even among children of Mexican American immigrants raised in poor households, the so-called immigrant advantage. To weigh the extent to which Mexican heritage or foreign-born status contributes to early growth, we first compare levels of cognitive and communicative skills among chil… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
9
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 19 publications
(9 citation statements)
references
References 30 publications
0
9
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Indeed maternal education is among the most robust predictors of children's vocabulary development (Magnuson, Sexton, Davis-Kean, & Huston, 2009). Children in homes with less educated mothers experience less quality language input, less verbal interactions, fewer cognitively stimulating activities, limited sentence structures, fewer learning materials, and less maternal contingent responding (Coddington, Mistry, & Bailey, 2014;Fuller et al, 2015;Hoff, 2013;Magnuson et al, 2009;Marcella et al, 2014;Snow, Burns, & Griffin, 1998). Negative associations between low maternal education and children's developmental outcomes, although similar between Caucasian and Latino mothers, are more pronounced in Spanish-speaking homes (Guerrero et al, 2013).…”
Section: Poverty and Maternal Educationmentioning
confidence: 96%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Indeed maternal education is among the most robust predictors of children's vocabulary development (Magnuson, Sexton, Davis-Kean, & Huston, 2009). Children in homes with less educated mothers experience less quality language input, less verbal interactions, fewer cognitively stimulating activities, limited sentence structures, fewer learning materials, and less maternal contingent responding (Coddington, Mistry, & Bailey, 2014;Fuller et al, 2015;Hoff, 2013;Magnuson et al, 2009;Marcella et al, 2014;Snow, Burns, & Griffin, 1998). Negative associations between low maternal education and children's developmental outcomes, although similar between Caucasian and Latino mothers, are more pronounced in Spanish-speaking homes (Guerrero et al, 2013).…”
Section: Poverty and Maternal Educationmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…Ecocultural theory sheds light on how families adaptively change (e.g., child-rearing goals, maternal practices, family structure) to fit the social context (e.g., school demands) as they respond to new and changing circumstances (Bridges et al, 2015;Fuller et al, 2015;Reese & Gallimore, 2000). In Latino families, the linkages between literacy socialization goals and schooling can depart from those in White families (who routinely engage children in language and literacy practices that mirror school-like formats) in ways that affect language-vocabulary specifically-prior to schooling (Fuller & García Coll, 2010).…”
Section: The Ecology Of Language Developmentmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Children living in households experiencing adversity have parents who are frequently less responsive and more intrusive (Dyer, Owen, & Caughty, ; Fuller, Bein, Kim, & Rabe‐Hesketh, ; Hart & Risley, ). Teenage mothers, for example, are often less verbally responsive and more intrusive than older mothers (Keown et al., ; Rafferty, Griffin, & Lodise, ).…”
Section: Maternal Responsiveness and Child Languagementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Teenage mothers, for example, are often less verbally responsive and more intrusive than older mothers (Keown et al., ; Rafferty, Griffin, & Lodise, ). Adversity, as used in the current study, refers to typical socioeconomic status measures but also includes other risk factors related to child language, for example, teen parenthood (Keown et al., ; Rafferty et al., ), ethnic minority status (Fuller et al., ), and parental mental health difficulties (Pan, Rowe, Singer, & Snow, ). Importantly, risk factors which impact child development are not mutually exclusive and may in fact cumulatively impact a parent's ability to cope (Conger, Conger, & Elder, ; Ghate & Hazel, ).…”
Section: Maternal Responsiveness and Child Languagementioning
confidence: 99%