1968
DOI: 10.1044/jshr.1103.622
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Disfluency Behavior of Elementary School Stutterers and Non-Stutterers: The Adaptation Effect

Abstract: A group of 184 elementary school children, 92 stutterers and 92 matched nonstutterers, performed a speaking task three times consecutively. Kindergarten and first grade children repeated a series of sentences, and the second through sixth grade children read a passage. Both the stutterers and the nonstutterers exhibited the adaptation effect. Both adapted proportionally to approximately the same degree. There was no tendency in either group for the degree of adaptation to vary as a function of grade level. Whe… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
12
0
1

Year Published

1982
1982
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
4
3
2

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 79 publications
(14 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
1
12
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…= 6.69). Similar indices of stuttering have been reported elsewhere (e.g., Yairi & Ambrose, 1992;Yairi & Lewis, 1984), with the specific definition of the "constituent members" or different disfluency types representative of stuttering also described elsewhere (Williams, Silverman, & Kools, 1968). …”
Section: Children Who Stuttermentioning
confidence: 77%
“…= 6.69). Similar indices of stuttering have been reported elsewhere (e.g., Yairi & Ambrose, 1992;Yairi & Lewis, 1984), with the specific definition of the "constituent members" or different disfluency types representative of stuttering also described elsewhere (Williams, Silverman, & Kools, 1968). …”
Section: Children Who Stuttermentioning
confidence: 77%
“…Given that the C-E model suggests that temperamental factors may exacerbate CWS’ speech disfluencies, a better understanding of the role of the temperamental characteristics in the development of stuttering might result from assessing how BI impacts the quantity of stuttered disfluencies within the group of CWS as well as CWNS. In the same vein, it would be interesting to investigate how BI impacts non-stuttered disfluencies (e.g., revisions) within CWS as well as CWNS, thus addressing the suggestion that there may be similar underlying mechanisms for stuttered and non-stuttered disfluencies (Johnson, 1959; Postma & Kolk, 1993; Williams, Silverman, & Kools, 1968). …”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The student was trained by the first author to recognize types of disfluency and secondary behaviors. The student was also given a standard definition of stuttering that included disfluency types suggested by Williams, Silverman, and Kools (1968). Disfluencies were defined as stuttering (part-word repetitions, disrhythmic phonationsprolongations, tense pauses) and non-stuttering (word repetitions, phrase repetitions, interjections, revisions) types (Johnson & Associates, 1959).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%