2009
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0006183
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Dissociable Perceptual Effects of Visual Adaptation

Abstract: Neurons in the visual cortex are responsive to the presentation of oriented and curved line segments, which are thought to act as primitives for the visual processing of shapes and objects. Prolonged adaptation to such stimuli gives rise to two related perceptual effects: a slow change in the appearance of the adapting stimulus (perceptual drift), and the distortion of subsequently presented test stimuli (adaptational aftereffects). Here we used a psychophysical nulling technique to dissociate and quantify the… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

1
17
0

Year Published

2011
2011
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(18 citation statements)
references
References 61 publications
1
17
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In the retina, a rapid process that occurs within 100 ms of a stimulus differs in mechanism from longerterm adaptation (28), and similar, dissociable, very rapid adaptation is evident in motion-selective area MT, where it plays a role in motion perception (29). The prior behavioral work reviewed above is also consistent with separate shorter-term mechanisms of contrast adaptation acting over seconds and minutes (22,23), as is additional behavioral work suggesting that contrast adaptation may be controlled by multiple mechanisms, but that was not concerned with the temporal properties of those mechanisms (30). Hence, the adaptation measured here likely represents the action of mechanisms near the long end of a continuous distribution across timescales.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 79%
“…In the retina, a rapid process that occurs within 100 ms of a stimulus differs in mechanism from longerterm adaptation (28), and similar, dissociable, very rapid adaptation is evident in motion-selective area MT, where it plays a role in motion perception (29). The prior behavioral work reviewed above is also consistent with separate shorter-term mechanisms of contrast adaptation acting over seconds and minutes (22,23), as is additional behavioral work suggesting that contrast adaptation may be controlled by multiple mechanisms, but that was not concerned with the temporal properties of those mechanisms (30). Hence, the adaptation measured here likely represents the action of mechanisms near the long end of a continuous distribution across timescales.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 79%
“…Some aftereffects, such as those induced by tilt adaptation, seem to arise primarily from contrastive processes, in which adaptation exaggerates differences between the adaptor and subsequent other stimuli without changing the adaptor's appearance (Mitchell and Muir (1976); but see Müller, Schillinger, Do, and Leopold (2009) and Chapter 2). Other aftereffects, such as those induced by adaptation to colour saturation, seem to arise from renormalisation processes in which a prolonged adaptor comes to appear more 'neutral' during exposure.…”
Section: Renormalisation and Local Repulsion: Two Patterns Of Visual mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the domain of orientation perception, the consensus tips in favour of a multichannel representation of orientation, giving rise to a predominantly locally repulsive tilt aftereffect, but this remains controversial (see Müller, Schillinger, et al (2009) and Chapter 2). In the domain of shape perception, a norm-based theory of aspect ratio encoding has been proposed (Regan & Hamstra, 1992;Suzuki, 2005), although there is little relevant evidence from aftereffects to support or falsify this.…”
Section: The Present Thesismentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations