2014
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.08.010
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Dissociating position and heading estimations: Rotated visual orientation cues perceived after walking reset headings but not positions

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

6
38
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2025
2025

Publication Types

Select...
6
3
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 23 publications
(44 citation statements)
references
References 37 publications
6
38
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In an environment with stable visual landmarks for piloting, the path integrator actually shuts down, so the navigator is completely disoriented if landmarks unexpectedly vanish (Zhao and Warren, 2015a). Moreover, familiar visual landmarks act to reset the path integrator (both orientation and position) in humans (Mou and Zhang, 2014;Zhang and Mou, 2017;Zhao and Warren, 2015b) as in animals (Etienne et al, 2004;Knierim et al, 1998). Such a system is well suited for making local, piecewise measurements of rough travel distances and turn angles and registering them in a cognitive graph.…”
Section: Building a Cognitive Graphmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In an environment with stable visual landmarks for piloting, the path integrator actually shuts down, so the navigator is completely disoriented if landmarks unexpectedly vanish (Zhao and Warren, 2015a). Moreover, familiar visual landmarks act to reset the path integrator (both orientation and position) in humans (Mou and Zhang, 2014;Zhang and Mou, 2017;Zhao and Warren, 2015b) as in animals (Etienne et al, 2004;Knierim et al, 1998). Such a system is well suited for making local, piecewise measurements of rough travel distances and turn angles and registering them in a cognitive graph.…”
Section: Building a Cognitive Graphmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Conversely, in a similar experiment by Zhao and Warren [24], participants primarily used visual allothetic information, often ignoring body-based idiothetic cues even when the mismatch was as large as 90 • , consistent with a cue competition strategy. Similar discrepancies exist across the literature, with some studies supporting cue combination (and even optimal Bayesian cue combination) [23][24][25][26][27], and others more consistent with cue competition [24,[28][29][30][31].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 61%
“…Although visual and self-motion cues may be optimally integrated to reduce the variability of spatial judgments (e.g., Chen et al 2017;Nardini et al 2008), these cues often compete to determine the direction in which a navigator should go (Tcheang et al 2011;Zhao & Warren 2015b). Visual cues often "veto" self-motion cues when they provide conflicting estimates of orientation or location; when such conflict becomes substantially large, the dominance reverts to self-motion cues (Foo et al 2005;Mou & Zhang 2014;Zhang & Mou 2017;Zhao & Warren 2015b; see Cheng et al 2007 for a review). This competition between visual and selfmotion information occurs in both human and nonhuman animal navigation and manifests in terms of both behavioral and neurophysiological responses (e.g., Etienne & Jeffery 2004;Yoder et al 2011).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%