“…For example, S1 sends excitatory axonal projections to M1 (Hooks et al, 2011;Mao et al, 2011;Rocco-Donovan et al, 2011), ablating S1 reduces or abolishes activity in M1 (Goldring et al, 1970;Farkas et al, 1999), silencing S1 impairs motor adaptation (Sakamoto et al, 1989;Mathis et al, 2017), and the latency of evoked and spontaneous sensory responses is typically shorter in S1 than M1 (Ferezou et al, 2007;Chakrabarti et al, 2008;McVea et al, 2012;An et al, 2014). However, the existence of short-latency sensory responses in M1 (Asanuma et al, 1979;Horne and Tracey, 1979;Lemon and van der Burg, 1979;Tracey et al, 1980;Herman et al, 1985;Asanuma and Mackel, 1989) and evidence of S1's role in motor control (Sasaki and Gemba, 1984;Matyas et al, 2010;Halley et al, 2020) suggest that the conventional division of S1 and M1 into distinct sensory and motor areas, respectively, is questionable (Hatsopoulos and Suminski, 2011;Ebbesen et al, 2018).…”