2018
DOI: 10.1007/s00426-018-1004-9
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Do already grasped objects activate motor affordances?

Abstract: This study investigated whether in a stimulus-response compatibility (SRC) task affordance effects in response to picture of graspable objects emerge when these objects appear as already grasped. It also assessed whether the observed effects could be explained as due to spatial compatibility between the most salient part in the object/display and the hand of response rather than to action potentiation. To this aim, we conducted three behavioural experiments in which participants were required to discriminate t… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

3
24
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

4
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 17 publications
(27 citation statements)
references
References 56 publications
3
24
0
Order By: Relevance
“…That is, responses were faster when the position of the handle and the responding hand were spatially aligned as compared to when they were not. This result was replicated with neurophysiological measures (e.g., Kourtis and Vingerhoets, 2015) and obtained across different tasks (e.g., Tipper et al, 2006;Saccone et al, 2016), stimuli (e.g., Pellicano et al, 2010;Pappas, 2014;Iani et al, 2018;Scerrati et al, 2019Scerrati et al, , 2020, populations (e.g., Dekker and Mareschal, 2013), response devices (e.g., Bub and Masson, 2010), and response modes (e.g., Phillips and Ward, 2002;Cho and Proctor, 2010;Proctor et al, 2017;Bub et al, 2018; for a review see Proctor and Miles, 2014; for a recent meta-analysis see Azaad et al, 2019).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 71%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…That is, responses were faster when the position of the handle and the responding hand were spatially aligned as compared to when they were not. This result was replicated with neurophysiological measures (e.g., Kourtis and Vingerhoets, 2015) and obtained across different tasks (e.g., Tipper et al, 2006;Saccone et al, 2016), stimuli (e.g., Pellicano et al, 2010;Pappas, 2014;Iani et al, 2018;Scerrati et al, 2019Scerrati et al, , 2020, populations (e.g., Dekker and Mareschal, 2013), response devices (e.g., Bub and Masson, 2010), and response modes (e.g., Phillips and Ward, 2002;Cho and Proctor, 2010;Proctor et al, 2017;Bub et al, 2018; for a review see Proctor and Miles, 2014; for a recent meta-analysis see Azaad et al, 2019).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 71%
“…Practice trials, reaction times (RTs) faster or slower than 2 SD from the participant’s mean (2.7% of the total trials) and errors (3.4% of the total trials) were excluded from the analysis on RTs. For simplicity and in line with previous studies (e.g., Iani et al, 2018 ), data were collapsed based on the spatial compatibility between handle orientation and response position (handle-response compatible vs. handle-response incompatible trials).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…That is, responses were faster when the position of the handle and the responding hand were spatially aligned as compared to when they were not. This result was replicated across different tasks (e.g., Tipper et al, 2006;Saccone et al, 2016), stimuli (e.g., Pellicano et al, 2010;Pappas, 2014;Iani et al, 2018;Scerrati et al, 2019Scerrati et al, , 2020, populations (e.g., Dekker and Mareschal, 2013), response devices (e.g., Bub and Masson, 2010), and response modes (e.g., Phillips and Ward, 2002;Cho and Proctor, 2010;Proctor et al, 2017;Bub et al, 2018; for a review see Proctor and Miles, 2014; for a recent meta-analysis see Azaad et al, 2019).…”
Section: A Corrigendum Onmentioning
confidence: 78%