2016
DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2016.08.004
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Do young children accept responsibility for the negative actions of ingroup members?

Abstract: This study investigated whether young children accept responsibility for the negative actions of ingroup members. Five-year-old children watched a transgressor break someone else's valued possession. Depending on condition, this transgressor either belonged to the same group as the child or a different group from the child. Coding of children's nonverbal behaviour indicated that they displayed more signs of guilt (but not other negative emotions) when the transgressor belonged to their own group than the other… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
14
0
2

Year Published

2018
2018
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1
1

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 21 publications
(16 citation statements)
references
References 32 publications
0
14
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Another possibility is that children might have wanted to protect their ingroup from the potential negative consequences (e.g., punishment) of their whistleblowing and/or avoid being punished themselves. Previous work has shown that children feel responsible for their group members' negative actions (Over et al, 2016 ), and consequently children's feelings of shame or embarrassment about their group members' transgression might have decreased their whistleblowing in the ingroup severe condition. Relatedly, some children might have been shocked about their group's transgression and thus too preoccupied to speak out about it.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Another possibility is that children might have wanted to protect their ingroup from the potential negative consequences (e.g., punishment) of their whistleblowing and/or avoid being punished themselves. Previous work has shown that children feel responsible for their group members' negative actions (Over et al, 2016 ), and consequently children's feelings of shame or embarrassment about their group members' transgression might have decreased their whistleblowing in the ingroup severe condition. Relatedly, some children might have been shocked about their group's transgression and thus too preoccupied to speak out about it.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Yet, this argument overlooks the possibility that at least some of children's beliefs about groups exist as part of a more abstract framework of how social groups function, beyond children's own experiences. For example, Tomasello reviews a great deal of work showing that preschool-aged children show preferential treatment for in-group over out-group members across a range of experimental paradigms (Fehr et al 2008;Misch et al 2014;Over 2018;Over et al 2016). These findings are not trivial and provide strong support for the suggestion that children's beliefs about obligation are embedded within their understanding of social groups.…”
Section: Intuitive Theories Inform Children's Beliefs About Intergroumentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Preschool children preferentially help in-group over out-group members (Over 2018). When a member of a 5-year-old's minimally established in-group does something mean to a victim, she feels an in-group responsibility to make amends to that victim – whereas she does not feel a responsibility if the perpetrator was an out-group member (Over et al 2016). When children in this age range are given a chance to share with children in their in-group, they do so relatively generously, whereas they do not do so with children from an out-group (Fehr et al 2008).…”
Section: A Shared Intentionality Accountmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, Bennett and colleagues (1998) found that when shown instances of unfamiliar ingroup members acting negatively, 7-year-olds, but not 5-yearolds, felt responsible for and embarrassed by these negative actions. Related research found that 5-year-old children already display subtler signs of guilt and responsibility after they observed their ingroup members break someone else's possession (Over, Vaish, & Tomasello, 2016). Furthermore, by age 5, children view themselves as particularly similar to children of the same gender if their gender identity has been made salient (Bennett & Sani, 2008a), and they recall words associated with their own groups (groups based on family, age, and gender) better than unrelated words, and to the same degree that they recall words associated with the self (Bennett & Sani, 2008b).…”
Section: How Social Categorization Influences Children's View Of Themselvesmentioning
confidence: 99%