2010
DOI: 10.1121/1.3478786
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Does fundamental-frequency discrimination measure virtual pitch discrimination?

Abstract: Studies of pitch perception often involve measuring difference limens for complex tones ͑DLCs͒ that differ in fundamental frequency ͑F0͒. These measures are thought to reflect F0 discrimination and to provide an indirect measure of subjective pitch strength. However, in many situations discrimination may be based on cues other than the pitch or the F0, such as differences in the frequencies of individual components or timbre ͑brightness͒. Here, DLCs were measured for harmonic and inharmonic tones under various… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

10
24
3

Year Published

2012
2012
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 23 publications
(37 citation statements)
references
References 45 publications
10
24
3
Order By: Relevance
“…Importantly, sensitivity to individual components would not necessarily require them to be in a harmonic relationship. A series of experiments that measured the change in F0DLs for inharmonic versus harmonic sounds found, where both groups had the same average resolvability and spectral range, that F0DLs were higher under the inharmonic condition (21,31,32). These results have been taken as evidence of a role of harmonicity in lowering F0DLs and, because F0DL for RESs is affected by changes to the fidelity of harmonicity, suggest that F0DL reflects sensitivity to a globally assembled pitch.…”
Section: Significancementioning
confidence: 84%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Importantly, sensitivity to individual components would not necessarily require them to be in a harmonic relationship. A series of experiments that measured the change in F0DLs for inharmonic versus harmonic sounds found, where both groups had the same average resolvability and spectral range, that F0DLs were higher under the inharmonic condition (21,31,32). These results have been taken as evidence of a role of harmonicity in lowering F0DLs and, because F0DL for RESs is affected by changes to the fidelity of harmonicity, suggest that F0DL reflects sensitivity to a globally assembled pitch.…”
Section: Significancementioning
confidence: 84%
“…3A illustrates these spectral shifts. Generally, any inharmonic shift should produce a more ambiguous pitch compared with the harmonic condition (21). Fig.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Such a correction was applied so that the audibility of the complex tones was approximately the same in all tested spectral regions. 3 In order to evaluate pitch salience, discrimination thresholds for equal frequency shifts of all stimulus components were predicted from the pitch matches, using an approach based on estimation theory (Edgeworth, 1908) and described in Micheyl et al (2010). The d 0 values predicted by this procedure can be used as an estimate of pitch salience.…”
Section: Overall Summary and Conclusionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Yet another example of situations in which the Gaussian assumption is inadequate relates to experiments in which a stimulus feature, which could provide an unwanted cue for task performance, is randomly perturbed (or "roved") across observation intervals within a trial, or across trials, in order to limit its use by participants. Examples of this abound in both the auditory-perception literature (e.g., Berg & Green, 1990;Berliner & Durlach, 1973;Dai, 1994Dai, , 2008Dai & Green, 1993;Dai, Nguyen, & Green, 1995Hall & Fernandes, 1983;Henning, 1966;Jesteadt & Bilger, 1974;Kidd, Mason, Brantley, & Owen, 1989;Kidd, Mason, Uchanski, Brantley, & Shah, 1991;Micheyl, Divis, Wrobleski, & Oxenham, 2010;Moore & Glasberg, 1989;Oxenham & Buus, 2000;Semal & Demany, 2006;Spiegel, Picardi, & Green, 1981;Strickland & Dhar, 2000) and the visual-perception literature (e.g., Danilova & Mollon, 2010;Dannemiller & Stephens, 1998;Morgan, 2005;Nachmias, 1999Nachmias, , 2002Regan & Hamstra, 1991, 1992Webster, De Valois, & Switkes, 1990). In such "roving" experiments, the experimenter must determine the size of the roving range necessary to ensure that the highest Pc that participants can achieve using the unwanted cue is below a predefined target (e.g., 60 % correct).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%