Highly acudcmu environments b e little bcn@for children's academic skills, muy dampen creative cxprcssion, and muy create some anxicty. These $ects, havcvcr, ure lcss dram& than some h e claimed.
Pressure or Challenge in Preschool?How Academic Environments Affect Children
Kathy Hirsh-PasekBloom (1964) writes that over 50 percent of adult intelligence is acquired by the age of four years. Assuming a causal relationship between this early knowledge and later intellectual ability, many argue that we should fill young minds full of information so that we can increase the intelligence of our children and can groom the next generation of leaders. Indeed, the past two decades of infancy research support this contention insofar as the work has revealed the striking abilities that infants and young toddlers have to process and learn new information.This line of reasoning represents the extreme position taken by those who endorse proposals for early academic orientations during the preschool years (Doman, 1964(Doman, , 1979(Doman, , 1984 Eastman and Barr. 1985; Engelmann and Engelmann, 1981; see Storfer, 1990, for a review). Academic attitudes and practices in the early years, they suggest, provide a challenge that enhances young minds and increases academic and intellectual abilities. By this account, both advantaged and disadvantaged children should benefit fiom this intellectual "head start."In direct opposition to this position are outspoken developmental psychologists and educators (Elkind, 1981(Elkind, , 1987 Sigel, 1987;Gallagher and Coche, 1987; Kagan and Zigler, 1987;Bredekamp, 1987) who argue that highly academic expectations and practices may harm young children by compromising n o d development Academic orientations for preschoolers, they suggest, offer "too much too soon" and constitute pressures that could result in social and emotional problems.