2018
DOI: 10.1177/0956797617754219
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Does Power Reduce Temporal Discounting? Commentary on Joshi and Fast (2013)

Abstract: Devaluing future outcomes, known as temporal discounting (Frederick, Loewenstein, & O'Donoghue, 2002), hinders one's ability to act in line with long-term over short-term interests. It is associated with maladaptive behaviors such as smoking (Kirby, Petry, & Bickel, 1999), drug use (Harrison, Lau, & Rutström, 2010), and not saving for retirement (Gubler & Pierce, 2014). Because temporal discounting may affect a variety of behaviors, ranging from exercising to energy consumption (Urminsky & Zauberman, 2015), un… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
10
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 13 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 50 publications
0
10
0
Order By: Relevance
“…However, that study does not use a recall-based power manipulation, but instead uses power posing, which is quite different (Ranehil, Dreber, Johannesson, Leiberg, Sul, & Weber, 2015). 2 Second, M&L cite research by Zhang and Smith (2018), to further support the argument that recall-based manipulations are unreliable. However, these authors actually argue in favor of the effectiveness of the recall-based power manipulation 3 .…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 96%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…However, that study does not use a recall-based power manipulation, but instead uses power posing, which is quite different (Ranehil, Dreber, Johannesson, Leiberg, Sul, & Weber, 2015). 2 Second, M&L cite research by Zhang and Smith (2018), to further support the argument that recall-based manipulations are unreliable. However, these authors actually argue in favor of the effectiveness of the recall-based power manipulation 3 .…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…3 Zhang and Smith (2018) report an unsuccessful replication of Joshi and Fast (2013) and conclude that although the recall manipulation does reliably induce a sense of power, it does not reliably affect the specific effect postulated by Joshi and Fast.…”
mentioning
confidence: 96%
“…This is especially relevant as some of the “effective” trainings and manipulations in this systematic review have failed to replicate in more recent studies. For example, Zhang and Smith (2018) did not find an effect of power on DD in two preregistered, close-replication studies of Joshi and Fast’s study in 2013; Sweeney et al (2018) failed to replicate the effects of a working memory training on DD in adolescents with cannabis use disorders; and Kable et al (2017) showed that cognitive training (including working memory training) was unable to decrease DD in healthy controls. We recommend adopting a basic set of principles and methods based on transparency and replicability guidelines (Miguel et al, 2014; Simmons et al, 2011) which would upgrade the quality of studies in general and make it easier to perform replication studies and compare across studies.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…K. Smith, Wigboldus, & Dijksterhuis, 2008; Wakslak, Smith, & Han, 2014; Wiesenfeld, Reyt, Brockner, & Trope, 2017). More pertinently, some initial research suggests that one’s hierarchical position may modulate regulatory scope: Joshi and Fast (2013) reported that people assigned to high-power versus low-power roles were more likely to prioritize larger, later rewards over smaller immediate rewards (see Zhang & Smith, 2018). Similar dynamics may characterize leaders versus followers.…”
Section: Beyond the Individual: Social Tools Support Expansive And Comentioning
confidence: 99%