2017
DOI: 10.1007/s00265-017-2398-x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Does similarity in call structure or foraging ecology explain interspecific information transfer in wild Myotis bats?

Abstract: Animals can gain important information by attending to the signals and cues of other animals in their environment, with acoustic information playing a major role in many taxa. Echolocation call sequences of bats contain information about the identity and behaviour of the sender which is perceptible to close-by receivers. Increasing evidence supports the communicative function of echolocation within species, yet data about its role for interspecific information transfer is scarce. Here, we asked which informati… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
25
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
2

Relationship

2
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 27 publications
(25 citation statements)
references
References 85 publications
0
25
0
Order By: Relevance
“…To estimate the localization error caused by errors in c , I generated the call sequences that a microphone array would receive from a bat at different spatial positions, and then analyzed these sequences with a different c than used during generation. Specifically, I used cross‐correlation to calculated TOADs for symmetrical planar star‐shaped four‐microphone arrays with 60 cm (e.g., Goerlitz, ter Hofstede et al., ; Hügel et al., ; Lewanzik & Goerlitz, ) and 120 cm intermicrophone distance, a c of 338 m/s, and a grid (2 m spacing) of bat positions filling half a hemisphere above the array up to 20 m distance ( x = 0–20, y = −20 to 20, z = 2–20; Figure ). The half‐hemisphere left of the array ( x = −20 to 0) was omitted as it is identical to the right one ( x = 0–20) due to the array's symmetry.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…To estimate the localization error caused by errors in c , I generated the call sequences that a microphone array would receive from a bat at different spatial positions, and then analyzed these sequences with a different c than used during generation. Specifically, I used cross‐correlation to calculated TOADs for symmetrical planar star‐shaped four‐microphone arrays with 60 cm (e.g., Goerlitz, ter Hofstede et al., ; Hügel et al., ; Lewanzik & Goerlitz, ) and 120 cm intermicrophone distance, a c of 338 m/s, and a grid (2 m spacing) of bat positions filling half a hemisphere above the array up to 20 m distance ( x = 0–20, y = −20 to 20, z = 2–20; Figure ). The half‐hemisphere left of the array ( x = −20 to 0) was omitted as it is identical to the right one ( x = 0–20) due to the array's symmetry.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The speed of sound (c) is the speed by which sounds propagate through the air (often approximated by 340 m/s). It is the fundamental physical sound parameter by which bats compute object range based on echo delay, as well as the fundamental parameter underlying the acoustic localization of echolocating bats and other soundproducing animals (reviewed in Blumstein et al, 2011), a method that is increasingly used to obtain bats' spatial positions based on the differences in arrival time of the same call on multiple microphones (e.g., Fujioka, Aihara, Sumiya, Aihara, & Hiryu, 2016;Goerlitz, ter Hofstede, Zeale, Jones, & Holderied, 2010;Hügel et al, 2017;Lewanzik & Goerlitz, 2018;Seibert, Koblitz, Denzinger, & Schnitzler, 2013;Surlykke, Pedersen, & Jakobsen, 2009).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We used custom‐made software (field: see Hügel et al., ; flight room: TOADSuite, Peter Stilz) to calculate the three‐dimensional position of the bat for each recorded call based on time‐of‐arrival differences between the four array microphones. Bat positions were manually combined into flight trajectories and smoothed, using cubic smoothing splines (see Figure b for exemplary trajectory).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The three remaining methods leveraged amplitude information to detect vocalizations, including an amplitude threshold within the frequency band of the target species (Simmons et al, 2008), a system that discarded extraneous low amplitude wind noise and used template matching to identify vocalizations in the remaining audio (Wang, Elson, Estrin, & Yao, 2003), and an amplitude‐detecting algorithm capable of adapting to continuously changing noise levels (Trifa et al, 2007). Curation methods for automated detectors included manually identifying calls that were not detected by automated detectors (e.g., Hügel et al, 2017) and removing false‐positive detections (e.g., Ali et al, 2007; Araya‐Salas et al, 2017). Another common manual curation step was excluding undesirable sounds from the target species, such as vocalizations with poor signal‐to‐noise ratios (e.g., Mennill et al, 2012; Papin et al, 2018; Sumiya et al, 2017) or vocalizations that were overlapped by the sounds of other species (e.g., Holderied, 2006; Krakauer et al, 2009).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although the set of sounds to localize was detected automatically in many studies, even nominally automated detection methods often required human curation in practice. Curation methods included finding calls that were not detected by automated detectors (e.g., Hügel et al, 2017), and excluding detections that were false positives (e.g., Ali et al, 2007; Araya‐Salas et al, 2017), had poor signal‐to‐noise ratios (e.g., Mennill et al, 2012; Papin et al, 2018; Sumiya et al, 2017), or were overlapped by other vocalizations (e.g., Holderied, 2006; Krakauer et al, 2009). Designing an automated detector requires a priori knowledge of species acoustic properties and becomes more challenging as the number of species to be analyzed increases.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%