2006
DOI: 10.1016/j.pragma.2005.12.005
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Does understanding negation entail affirmation?

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

15
120
1

Year Published

2006
2006
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 198 publications
(136 citation statements)
references
References 28 publications
15
120
1
Order By: Relevance
“…These results confirmed our hypothesis that the RTs for all modified antonyms were longer than for the bare antonyms, but at the same time they indicated that the negator is more complex to process than ganska. These tendencies for response times were also confirmed by Kaup and Zwaan (2003), MacDonald and Just (1989), Giora et al (2005a), Hasson and Glucksberg (2006) and Kaup et al (2006).…”
Section: Unbounded and Bounded Antonyms With Degree Modifierssupporting
confidence: 56%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…These results confirmed our hypothesis that the RTs for all modified antonyms were longer than for the bare antonyms, but at the same time they indicated that the negator is more complex to process than ganska. These tendencies for response times were also confirmed by Kaup and Zwaan (2003), MacDonald and Just (1989), Giora et al (2005a), Hasson and Glucksberg (2006) and Kaup et al (2006).…”
Section: Unbounded and Bounded Antonyms With Degree Modifierssupporting
confidence: 56%
“…From the point of view of function, Giora (2006) claims that suppression and retention are pragmatically motivated on all occasions rather than being automatic mechanisms. Hasson and Glucksberg (2006) and Kaup et al (2006) assume a juxtapositon of the negated state of affairs with the representation of the described situation which suggests that both construals are needed for our understanding of negated meanings.…”
Section: The Suppression Versus the Retention Hypothesismentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Most research has compared affirmative and negative sentences in isolation (Hasson & Glucksberg, 2006;Just & Carpenter, 1971;Kaup et al, 2006;McDonald & Just, 1989;Wason, 1961), or following a supportive context (e.g. Giora, Fein, Aschkenazi & Alkabets-Zlozover, 2007;Glenberg, Robertson, Jansen & Johnson-Glenberg, 1999;Wason, 1965) and as such cannot address the issue of how a negated concept is reflected on-line in later processing of relevant information.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Voluminous behavioral research shows that it is more complicated and effortful to represent negation than affirmation (Evans, Newstead, & Byrne, 1993;Gough, 1966;Hasson & Glucksberg, 2006;Kaup, 2001;MacDonald & Just, 2002;Slobin, 1966;Stupple & Waterhouse, 2009;Wason, 1965;Watson, 1961), and recent work in cognitive neuroscience has begun uncovering the neural signatures of this difference (Christensen, 2009;Kumar, Padakannaya, Mishra, & Khetrapal, 2013). Accordingly, one might expect knowledge ascriptions for positive claims to be higher in general, whether the source of belief is inference or perception, because "any variable that increases experienced ease of processing is also likely to increase judgments of truth" (Reber & Schwarz, 1999: 342).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%