2001
DOI: 10.1016/s0885-2014(02)00070-9
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Domain effects in lexical development: learning words for foods and toys

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

5
59
1

Year Published

2008
2008
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 40 publications
(65 citation statements)
references
References 24 publications
5
59
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Indeed, children under 2 y of age are the most likely age group to accidentally poison themselves (24). Additionally, although adults, older children, and even adult monkeys rely on different perceptual properties to make inferences about foods and about artifacts (e.g., using color when reasoning about foods, shape when reasoning about artifacts), human infants do not (5,(27)(28)(29)(30)(31)(32)(33)(34)(35). Because human infants have historically relied on caregivers to provide safe and nutritious diets, they may not need to have mechanisms in place for reasoning about food.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Indeed, children under 2 y of age are the most likely age group to accidentally poison themselves (24). Additionally, although adults, older children, and even adult monkeys rely on different perceptual properties to make inferences about foods and about artifacts (e.g., using color when reasoning about foods, shape when reasoning about artifacts), human infants do not (5,(27)(28)(29)(30)(31)(32)(33)(34)(35). Because human infants have historically relied on caregivers to provide safe and nutritious diets, they may not need to have mechanisms in place for reasoning about food.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, color might be relevant to labeling within the basic-level category of zebras, but it is less relevant to labeling horses, dogs, birds, and so forth. In a similar way, the superordinate category of foods is organized by color and material, meaning it encompasses numerous basic-level categories of color-similar objects and/or material-similar objects (Lavin & Hall, 2002;Macario, 1991;Santos, Hauser, & Spelke, 2002). Thus, children cannot effectively rely on any single strategy-a shape bias, color bias, or otherwise-to learn labels for objects from all basic or superordinate categories.…”
Section: The Diversity Of Object Categoriesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although young children often over-map and over-generalize object labels by shape (Clark, 1973;Graham & Poulin-Dubois, 1999;Jones et al, 1991;Jones & Smith, 1998;Samuelson et al, 2008), their mapping behavior becomes more nuanced over time (Booth & Waxman, 2002;Imai, Gentner, & Uchida, 1994;Jones & Smith, 1998;Jones et al, 1991;Keil, 1994;Lavin & Hall, 2002;Macario, 1991;Samuelson et al, 2008;Santos et al, 2002). For example, Lavin and Hall (2001) demonstrated differences in threeyear-old children's mapping and generalization of labels for toys and foods.…”
Section: The Diversity Of Object Categoriesmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Using these methods, researchers of cognitive development have learned a lot about early cognitive competencies (e.g., Keil, 1989;Spelke, 1990;Mandler, 1992; Gelman, Coley, & Gottfried, 1994;Kemler Nelson, Frankenfield, Morris, & Blair, 2000;Bloom, 2000; Booth, Huang, & Waxman, 2005;Lavin & Hall, 2001). Nonetheless, this approach may be fundamentally flawed, both theoretically and empirically (see, Thelen & Smith, 1994;Smith, 2005).…”
Section: Flexibility and Variabilitymentioning
confidence: 99%