2012
DOI: 10.1016/j.joep.2012.01.005
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Drawing the line somewhere: An experimental study of moral compromise

Abstract: In a study by Shalvi, Dana, Handgraaf and De Dreu (2011) it was convincingly demonstrated that psychologically, the distinction between right and wrong is not discrete, rather it is a continuous distribution of relative 'rightness' and 'wrongness'. Using the 'die-underthe-cup' paradigm participants over-reported high numbers on the roll of a die when there were financial incentives to do so and no chance of detection for lying. Participants generally did not maximize income, instead making moral compromises. I… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

9
56
3
4

Year Published

2013
2013
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8
2

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 71 publications
(72 citation statements)
references
References 12 publications
9
56
3
4
Order By: Relevance
“…If participants do not cheat, all 6 options should be reported with the same frequency. Actually only 6.4% report 6, while 35.0% report 5, and 27.2% report 4 (a similar finding is Lewis, Bardis et al 2012). The authors conclude that there is dishonest reporting, but that many participants do not lie maximally.…”
Section: B) Fraudsupporting
confidence: 63%
“…If participants do not cheat, all 6 options should be reported with the same frequency. Actually only 6.4% report 6, while 35.0% report 5, and 27.2% report 4 (a similar finding is Lewis, Bardis et al 2012). The authors conclude that there is dishonest reporting, but that many participants do not lie maximally.…”
Section: B) Fraudsupporting
confidence: 63%
“…The ability to rationalize dishonest behavior is often a critical precursor to unethical behavior (Gino & Ariely, 2012;Lewis et al, 2012;Shalvi, Dana, Handgraaf, & De Dreu, 2011) and perceiving a counterpart as too incompetent to scrutinize deception may provide a better justification for deception than simply being too nice. These results suggest that deception in distributive negotiations may be a form of succumbing to moral temptation (Monin, Pizarro, & Beer, 2007) posed by the perception that a negotiating counterpart ''had it coming'' by lacking competence.…”
Section: Theoretical Contributionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…(Grover, 1993, p. 478) 4 Our methodology for measuring cheating is related to a recent wave of experimental studies that use a report of a privately-known outcome as an indicator of cheating or dishonesty. These papers use a 'hard' form of cheating or deception, where subjects report the outcome of a coin flip (Bucciol and Piovesan, 2011;Houser et al, 2012;Abeler et al, 2012), a die roll (Fischbacher and Heusi, 2008;Lammers et al, 2010;Conrads et al, 2013;Fischbacher and Utikal, 2011;Hao and Houser, 2011;Shalvi et al, 2011;Suri et al, 2011;Cojoc and Stoian, 2012;Lewis et al, 2012;Shalvi et al, forthcoming) or some other random variable (Coricelli et al, 2010;Eisenkopf et al, 2011). Some studies ask subjects to report their score on a task (e.g., Mazar et al, 2008;Cadsby et al, 2010;Schwieren and Weichselbaumer, 2010).…”
Section: Relevance Of Our Set-up To Labor Marketsmentioning
confidence: 99%