1996
DOI: 10.1016/0010-0277(95)00696-6
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Drinking and driving don't mix: inductive generalization in infancy

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2

Citation Types

18
176
1
2

Year Published

2000
2000
2007
2007

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 249 publications
(197 citation statements)
references
References 17 publications
18
176
1
2
Order By: Relevance
“…The available choices can be more or less related to the modeled act, and so the objects the infants choose provide information about what they have understood. For example, when we modeled a dog drinking from a cup and then gave infants the cup along with another animal and a vehicle, they almost always used the other animal for their imitations (Mandler & McDonough, 1996). This finding indicates that they understood that animals drink but vehicles do not.…”
mentioning
confidence: 93%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…The available choices can be more or less related to the modeled act, and so the objects the infants choose provide information about what they have understood. For example, when we modeled a dog drinking from a cup and then gave infants the cup along with another animal and a vehicle, they almost always used the other animal for their imitations (Mandler & McDonough, 1996). This finding indicates that they understood that animals drink but vehicles do not.…”
mentioning
confidence: 93%
“…In our most recent series of experiments investigating concept formation in infancy, we used a generalized imitation technique (Mandler & McDonough, 1996, 1998bMcDonough & Mandler, 1998; see also Bauer & Dow, 1994). We model a simple event using small replicas of real-world objects and encourage infants to imitate what they have observed.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…From the point of view of language acquisition, crosslinguistic studies of early language show that young children everywhere talk about simple events in which animate agents act on inanimate objects (e.g., Brown, 1973;Slobin, 1973Slobin, , 1985. Congruent with this finding, developmental research has shown that preverbal concepts of animacy, inanimacy, and agency begin to be formulated in the first year of life, as well as concepts of the events in which objects take part (Johnson, Slaughter, & Carey, 1998;Legerstee, 1992;Mandler & McDonough, 1996;Spelke, Phillips, & Woodward, 1995). These abstract characterizations of events provide a conceptual base onto which simple sentence structure can be mapped (Mandler, 1996).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A good deal of research has begun to flesh out the concepts preverbal infants have formed about animate and inanimate objects (e.g., Mandler & McDonough, 1996, 2000, but the concepts onto which spatial language is mapped have remained more speculative. Perhaps the best known approach was that of the Clarks (Clark, E., 1973;Clark, H., 1973), who assumed that there are cognitive universals that account not only for commonalities in spatial terms across various languages but also for the easy acquisition of simple spatial prepositions.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%