2023
DOI: 10.36850/e13
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Driven to Snack: Simulated Driving Increases Subsequent Consumption

Abstract: When individuals eat while distracted, they may compensate by consuming more afterwards. Here, we examined the effect of eating while driving, and explored potential underlying mechanisms. Participants (N = 116, 73.3% female) were randomly allocated to complete a driving simulation (distraction condition) or to watch someone else drive (control condition) while consuming 10g (50.8 kcal) of potato chips. Afterwards, participants rated the taste intensity and hedonic experience, reported stress levels, and were … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1

Citation Types

0
2
0

Year Published

2023
2023
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
3

Relationship

1
2

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 3 publications
(2 citation statements)
references
References 44 publications
0
2
0
Order By: Relevance
“…For instance, this link has been proposed to exist because being distracted renders people forgetful of personal health goals (e.g., to eat healthily; Tapper, 2017), less aware of satiation signals (Tapper, 2017), and less able to remember what (and how much) was eaten during a previous consumption episode (Higgs & Spetter, 2018). While the distracted eating research documented thus far has been valuable, distracted (over)eating has not yet been mechanistically connected to hedonic compensation (though for preliminary evidence, see van der Wal & van Dillen, 2013; van Meer, Murphy, et al, 2023). The distracted eating literature also overlooks the possibility that the distraction-consumption link is part of a larger story that involves all consumption domains (e.g., food, drink, gaming).…”
Section: Distraction and Actual Consumption Enjoymentmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…For instance, this link has been proposed to exist because being distracted renders people forgetful of personal health goals (e.g., to eat healthily; Tapper, 2017), less aware of satiation signals (Tapper, 2017), and less able to remember what (and how much) was eaten during a previous consumption episode (Higgs & Spetter, 2018). While the distracted eating research documented thus far has been valuable, distracted (over)eating has not yet been mechanistically connected to hedonic compensation (though for preliminary evidence, see van der Wal & van Dillen, 2013; van Meer, Murphy, et al, 2023). The distracted eating literature also overlooks the possibility that the distraction-consumption link is part of a larger story that involves all consumption domains (e.g., food, drink, gaming).…”
Section: Distraction and Actual Consumption Enjoymentmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Our secondary aim is to show that this tendency to compensate for a hedonic shortfall helps explain a widely reported effect in extant literature-the effect whereby distraction during food consumption promotes increased food consumption (Gonçalves et al, 2019;Higgs & Spetter, 2018;S. Marsh et al, 2013;Ogden et al, 2013;Robinson et al, 2013;van Meer, Murphy, et al, 2023). Much effort has been invested to better understand why distraction promotes increased food consumption (Higgs & Spetter, 2018;Tapper, 2017;van der Wal & van Dillen, 2013;van Dillen & van Steenbergen, 2018).…”
mentioning
confidence: 98%