1989
DOI: 10.3758/bf03210704
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Duration, time constant, and decay of the linear motion aftereffect as a function of inspection duration

Abstract: Subjects rated the strength of the motion aftereffect (MAE) produced by the upward motion of a horizontal grating in two experiments. Inspection periods ranged from 30 to 900 sec in Experiment 1 and from 20 to 120 sec in Experiment 2. A minimum of 22 h elapsed between trials. The decay time constant increased as the square root of the inspection duration for values between 1 min and 15 min of inspection. The ratings suggested that the MAEs consisted of three phases: an initial maximum-strength phase, a decay p… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

7
57
1

Year Published

1995
1995
2012
2012

Publication Types

Select...
5
3
2

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 67 publications
(65 citation statements)
references
References 21 publications
(32 reference statements)
7
57
1
Order By: Relevance
“…We see both a logarithmic increase in adaptation with action repetition and a logarithmic decrease with ISI, inconsistent with a simple priming effect, but consistent with studies investigating the dynamics of tilt (Magnussen & Johnsen, 1986), motion (Hershenson, 1989), face identity (Leopold, Rhodes, Muller, & Jeffery, 2005), face configuration (Rhodes et al, 2007), and biological motion aftereffects (Troje et al, 2006). As the dynamics of the hand action aftereffect follows this classic time course, it suggests that the adapted mechanism is perceptual in nature and neither an artifact of subject behavior during the experimental task, nor perhaps due to other postperceptual mechanisms.…”
Section: Hand Action Adaptation Dynamicssupporting
confidence: 83%
“…We see both a logarithmic increase in adaptation with action repetition and a logarithmic decrease with ISI, inconsistent with a simple priming effect, but consistent with studies investigating the dynamics of tilt (Magnussen & Johnsen, 1986), motion (Hershenson, 1989), face identity (Leopold, Rhodes, Muller, & Jeffery, 2005), face configuration (Rhodes et al, 2007), and biological motion aftereffects (Troje et al, 2006). As the dynamics of the hand action aftereffect follows this classic time course, it suggests that the adapted mechanism is perceptual in nature and neither an artifact of subject behavior during the experimental task, nor perhaps due to other postperceptual mechanisms.…”
Section: Hand Action Adaptation Dynamicssupporting
confidence: 83%
“…We term this relationship the duration scaling law, and it has been confirmed in prior work that measured contrast detection, the tilt aftereffect, the motion aftereffect, and adaptation that results from viewing a face (4,(12)(13)(14). These studies used short durations, generally a few minutes in length.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 70%
“…For example, most aftereffects display interocular transfer if the adapting and test eye are different (Gibson 1937;Wade et al 1993), exhibit storage across blank periods (Spigel 1960;Thompson & Movshon 1978), and are restricted to confined spatial zones in the visual field (Gibson 1937;Anstis & Gregory 1965). Also, aftereffects have a finite duration that depends upon adaptor strength and exposure time (Gibson & Radner 1937;Wolfe 1984;Magnussen & Greenlee 1987;Hershenson 1989).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%