2015
DOI: 10.1177/0023830915587038
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Dutch and German 3-Year-Olds’ Representations of Voicing Alternations

Abstract: The voicing contrast is neutralised syllable and word finally in Dutch and German, leading to alternations within the morphological paradigm (e.g. Dutch 'bed(s). Despite structural similarity, language-specific morphological, phonological and lexical properties impact on the distribution of this alternation in the two languages. Previous acquisition research has focused on one language only, predominantly focusing on children's production accuracy, concluding that alternations are not acquired until late in th… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

0
22
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(22 citation statements)
references
References 75 publications
0
22
0
Order By: Relevance
“…For the GAMM on experimental trials, we modeled empirical-logit-transformed fixations in the CROSS-SPLICE condition in order to assess sensitivity to phonetic mismatch. The time window of interest was chosen for analysis of experimental trials between 300 ms after word onset to account for eye movement programming delay (Buckler & Fikkert, 2016;Zamuner et al, 2016) until 2000 ms after word onset, a time at which it is likely that children will continue to look at the images based on the prompt. Similarly to other GAMM analyses (Porretta, Tucker & Järvikivi, 2016;Porretta et al, 2018), random effects corresponded to a combination of participant and trial (i.e., EVENT), allowing each trial (for each participant) to have its own intercept in the model.…”
Section: Analysis Proceduresmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For the GAMM on experimental trials, we modeled empirical-logit-transformed fixations in the CROSS-SPLICE condition in order to assess sensitivity to phonetic mismatch. The time window of interest was chosen for analysis of experimental trials between 300 ms after word onset to account for eye movement programming delay (Buckler & Fikkert, 2016;Zamuner et al, 2016) until 2000 ms after word onset, a time at which it is likely that children will continue to look at the images based on the prompt. Similarly to other GAMM analyses (Porretta, Tucker & Järvikivi, 2016;Porretta et al, 2018), random effects corresponded to a combination of participant and trial (i.e., EVENT), allowing each trial (for each participant) to have its own intercept in the model.…”
Section: Analysis Proceduresmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The window of analysis started 200 ms after the beginning of the target, accounting for the time it takes to initiate eye movements (Matin, Shao, & Boff, 1993;Salverda, Kleinschmidt, & Tanenhaus, 2014). Note that in studies with children, analyses can also begin 300 ms after the target, due to longer latencies in children's eye movements (Buckler & Fikkert, 2016; also see Swingley & Aslin, 2000, for a discussion of latencies). All analyses presented below were also done starting at 300 ms after word onset, with the same results.…”
Section: Analysesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This resulted in three trial orders. To make sure children would remain engaged in the task, four filler trials involved correct pronunciations of four well-known words (e.g., Singh et al, 2015 ; Buckler and Fikkert, 2016 ). Test phases across all versions started with a filler trial to help children understand the nature of the task.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Test trials were excluded if (1) a child looked less than 500 ms during the 2000 ms post-naming window (e.g., Quam and Swingley, 2010 ; Singh et al, 2014 ; Tsuji et al, 2016 ), (2) the participant fixated only one of two objects during the 2500 ms pre-naming window (e.g., White and Morgan, 2008 ; Mani and Plunkett, 2011 ; Singh et al, 2015 ; Buckler and Fikkert, 2016 ), (3) an equipment or experimenter error occurred, and (4) if a participant refused to participate (e.g., by getting up and walking around) and the experiment had to be aborted.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation