2001
DOI: 10.1016/s0167-8760(01)00139-8
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Dynamic behavior of the auditory N100 elicited by a baby's cry

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
12
0

Year Published

2006
2006
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 23 publications
(12 citation statements)
references
References 12 publications
0
12
0
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, it has been shown by event-related brain potential studies that auditory attention requires some of the parent response regions such as anterior cingulate and temporal cortices (Tzourio et al, 1997). However, in another study using similar techniques, women responded significantly more to a baby cry than to an emotionally neutral vocalization in these regions (Purhonen et al, 2001b); in a third study, mothers responded more than control women to infant cries (Purhonen et al, 2001a). These results suggest that increases in the mother’s alertness and arousal toward baby signals may function to assist the mother her ability to be continuously alert or attuned to the infant’s needs.…”
Section: Parental Brains and Baby Cry Stimulimentioning
confidence: 91%
“…For example, it has been shown by event-related brain potential studies that auditory attention requires some of the parent response regions such as anterior cingulate and temporal cortices (Tzourio et al, 1997). However, in another study using similar techniques, women responded significantly more to a baby cry than to an emotionally neutral vocalization in these regions (Purhonen et al, 2001b); in a third study, mothers responded more than control women to infant cries (Purhonen et al, 2001a). These results suggest that increases in the mother’s alertness and arousal toward baby signals may function to assist the mother her ability to be continuously alert or attuned to the infant’s needs.…”
Section: Parental Brains and Baby Cry Stimulimentioning
confidence: 91%
“…While a number of factors in adults, such as being a parent (Proverbio, Brignone, Matarazzo, Del Zotto, & Zani, 2006;Purhonen, Pääkkönen, Yppärilä, Lehtonen, & Karhu, 2001;Seifritz et al, 2003) and psychiatric disturbance (Laurent & Ablow, 2012;Schechter et al, 2011), have been shown to impact on brain responses to infant cues, it is likely that infant characteristics are equally important. Facial anomalies, such as cleft lip, where the structure deviates from the normal configuration, provide a window to understanding characteristics of the infant that can shape interactions with a caregiver or other adult.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This has included ERP studies examining very early perceptual processing of infant vocalizations and facial expressions, namely the N100 and N170. With respect to infant cry perception, maternal and non-maternal samples have been exposed to infant cry and control (i.e., a word) stimuli, and the N100 elicited by these stimuli was modulated by both the nature of the stimulus (cry vs. word) and maternal status (mother vs. non-mother; (Purhonen, Kilpeläinen-Lees, et al, 2001; Purhonen, Valkonen-Korhonen, & Lehtonen, 2008)). Further, with respect to early face perception, the N170 elicited by infant faces has also showed differences by maternal status (Proverbio, Brignone, Matarazzo, Del Zotto, & Zani, 2006).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%