2014
DOI: 10.1111/sode.12076
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Early and Middle Adolescents' Reasoning About Moral and Personal Concerns in Opposite‐sex Interactions

Abstract: This study examined how adolescents coordinate personal and moral concerns in reasoning about opposite‐sex interactions. Sixty‐four early and middle adolescents (Ms = 12.74, 16.05 years) were individually interviewed about two hypothetical situations involving opposite‐sex interactions (commenting on appearance, initiating a date), presented in four conditions that varied the salience of personal vs. moral concerns. Overall, participants viewed opposite‐sex interactions as harmless and acceptable in personal c… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1

Citation Types

0
8
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 8 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 25 publications
0
8
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Because multiple reasons were allowed, responses were scored in terms of the proportional use of each reason (e.g., a response referring to two reasons scored each reason .5). Assumptions of the consensual status of the practice were scored as 0 = ‘ nonconsensual ’ and 1 = ‘ consensual .’ Finally the contingency of the practice evaluation on the consensual status of the practice was scored as 0 = ‘ noncontingent ’ (i.e., the manipulation of the consensual status of the practice did not change the evaluation) or 1 = ‘ contingent ’ (i.e., the manipulation of the consensual status of the practice changed the evaluation; see Shaw, Wainryb, & Smetana, ).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Because multiple reasons were allowed, responses were scored in terms of the proportional use of each reason (e.g., a response referring to two reasons scored each reason .5). Assumptions of the consensual status of the practice were scored as 0 = ‘ nonconsensual ’ and 1 = ‘ consensual .’ Finally the contingency of the practice evaluation on the consensual status of the practice was scored as 0 = ‘ noncontingent ’ (i.e., the manipulation of the consensual status of the practice did not change the evaluation) or 1 = ‘ contingent ’ (i.e., the manipulation of the consensual status of the practice changed the evaluation; see Shaw, Wainryb, & Smetana, ).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…When a person judges that their sexual behaviors are acceptable, they have little reason to refrain from those behaviors or to condemn others for similar behaviors. Research on moral psychology and moral development offers a framework for explaining why a perpetrator may deem acceptable acts that from the victim’s perspective constitute sexual violations (Killen & Dahl, 2021; Shaw et al, 2014). Crucially, moral judgments derive not only from fundamental moral concerns with others’ welfare or rights but also from perceptions of how actions affect the experiences and opportunities of others.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This research indicated that consent was seen as necessary, though not sufficient for a social interaction to be judged to be morally permissible. Research on cross-gender interpersonal transgressions shows that consent is seen as key (Shaw, Wainryb, & Smetana, 2014). Shaw et al (2014) found that when different gender interactions were seen as entailing coercion, they were judged more negatively.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Research on cross-gender interpersonal transgressions shows that consent is seen as key (Shaw, Wainryb, & Smetana, 2014). Shaw et al (2014) found that when different gender interactions were seen as entailing coercion, they were judged more negatively.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation