1988
DOI: 10.1017/s0305000900012381
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Early talk about the past: the origins of conversational stories of personal experience

Abstract: This paper explores children's early talk about specific distant past events and its development into conversational stories of personal experience, with special focus on its evaluative function. The data consist of longitudinal home observations of five working-class children and their mothers from ages 2;o to 2;6. Results indicate that during this period the children talked primarily about negative past events, especially events of physical harm; the rate of talk about specific past events doubled; temporall… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
151
0
10

Year Published

1999
1999
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
5
3
2

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 272 publications
(165 citation statements)
references
References 14 publications
4
151
0
10
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, children may justify their requests or their oppositions (e.g. Dunn, 1991;Eisenberg, 1985;Veneziano & Sinclair, 1995;Veneziano 2001Veneziano , 2009) or talk about past events in co-constructed conversations (Eisenberg, 1985;Lucariello & Nelson, 1987;Miller & Sperry, 1988;Veneziano & Sinclair, 1995;Veneziano, 2009). Clearly these behaviors are still primitive, and become more elaborate only later.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, children may justify their requests or their oppositions (e.g. Dunn, 1991;Eisenberg, 1985;Veneziano & Sinclair, 1995;Veneziano 2001Veneziano , 2009) or talk about past events in co-constructed conversations (Eisenberg, 1985;Lucariello & Nelson, 1987;Miller & Sperry, 1988;Veneziano & Sinclair, 1995;Veneziano, 2009). Clearly these behaviors are still primitive, and become more elaborate only later.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In a longitudinal study, P. J. Miller and Sperry (1988) found that 1.5-to 2.5-year-old girls' talk with their mothers about distant past events was primarily about negative events, especially those involving physical harm. A longitudinal case study that examined a child's ability to talk with her mother about the past between 20 and 28 months (Hudson, 1991) revealed that both mother and daughter discussed past negative emotions far more than positive emotions: negative emotions comprised 68% of emotions mentioned by the mother and 76% of those mentioned by the daughter 5 .…”
Section: B Evidence From Other Developmental Domainsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Its development spans over a long time, from the beginnings when familiar partners scaffold children's first references to the past (e.g., Miller and Sperry, 1988;Sachs, 1983;Veneziano and Sinclair, 1995) to the first simple narratives of recurrent events and of personal experience (e.g., Nelson, 1999;Peterson and McCabe, 1991), through child-initiated autonomous personal life and fictional narratives whose structural organization and linguistic expression develop through the school years up until adolescence and even adulthood (Berman and Slobin, 1994;Hickmann, 1995;Berman, 2009). Research has shown that, at a given age, narrative competence may vary depending on the content and on the context in which children produce their narratives.…”
Section: Narrative Developmentmentioning
confidence: 99%