2011
DOI: 10.1037/a0022330
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Ease of processing constrains the activation flow in the conceptual-lexical system during speech planning.

Abstract: In 3 picture-word interference experiments, speakers named a target object in the presence of an unrelated not-to-be-named context object. Distractor words, which were phonologically related or unrelated to the context object's name, were used to determine whether the context object had become phonologically activated. All objects had high frequency names, and the ease of processing of these objects was manipulated by a visual degradation technique. In Experiment 1, both objects were nondegraded; in Experiment… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

4
17
0

Year Published

2012
2012
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 24 publications
(21 citation statements)
references
References 56 publications
(114 reference statements)
4
17
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Image agreement has not been systematically addressed in picture-naming studies, but those in which this variable has been taken into account have found it to exert a reliable influence (e.g., Barry et al, 1997;Bonin, Peereman, et al, 2003;Nishimoto, Ueda, Miyawaki, Une, & Takahashi, 2012). Our finding that word frequency was a reliable predictor of picture-naming speed is completely consistent with the view that object naming requires access to word form representations that are stored on the basis of their frequency of encounter (e.g., Jescheniak & Levelt, 1994;Mädebach, Jescheniak, Oppermann, & Schriefers, 2011).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 83%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Image agreement has not been systematically addressed in picture-naming studies, but those in which this variable has been taken into account have found it to exert a reliable influence (e.g., Barry et al, 1997;Bonin, Peereman, et al, 2003;Nishimoto, Ueda, Miyawaki, Une, & Takahashi, 2012). Our finding that word frequency was a reliable predictor of picture-naming speed is completely consistent with the view that object naming requires access to word form representations that are stored on the basis of their frequency of encounter (e.g., Jescheniak & Levelt, 1994;Mädebach, Jescheniak, Oppermann, & Schriefers, 2011).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 83%
“…Indeed, the key finding of our study is that objective word frequency was a reliable determinant of objectnaming but not of object comprehension times. This is especially important since objective word frequency has been acknowledged to be a genuine index of the retrieval of word forms or word form encoding (e.g., Bonin et al, 2012;Mädebach et al, 2011). However, AoA was also a reliable predictor of object comprehension times, and skeptical readers might consider this finding to be very much at odds with the idea that the name-object verification task is a reliable way of indexing the prelexical (but not the lexical) levels involved in object naming, given that AoA has often been considered to be located at the level of word form representations in object naming (for reviews, see Johnston & Barry, 2006;Juhasz, 2005).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Finally, semantic interference (Oppermann, Jescheniak, Schriefers, & Gorges, 2010) and phonological facilitation (Madebach, Jescheniak, Oppermann, & Schriefers, 2010) effects from adjacent but not superimposed objects have been observed. In these experiments, subjects were cued to name one picture and told to ignore the other.…”
mentioning
confidence: 95%
“…The difficulty of excluding distractor objects can arise because (a) distractor pictures were superimposed on target pictures (Morsella & Miozzo, 2002; Navarette & Costa, 2005; Tipper, 1985), (b) subjects were unable to anticipate before trial onset which object is the target and which is the distractor because of spatial (Glaser & Glaser, 1989; Madebach et al, 2010; Oppermann et al, 2010) or temporal (Humphreys, Lloyd-Jones, & Fias, 1995) uncertainty, or (c) targets and distractors were integrated into a thematically coherent scene (Oppermann et al, 2008). …”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…When they shifted their eye gaze to the second picture, this picture was replaced with a new picture. Facilitation was found when the old and new picture were identical or homophonous, suggesting that the second (old) picture was processed while the participants were still looking at the first picture (see also Mädebach, Jescheniak, Oppermann, & Schriefers, 2011;Malpass & Meyer, 2010;Morgan, Van Elswijk, & Meyer, 2008;Schotter, Ferreira, & Rayner, 2013).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%