2019
DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.09.049
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Eccentricity-dependent temporal contrast tuning in human visual cortex measured with fMRI

Abstract: Cells in the peripheral retina tend to have higher contrast sensitivity and respond at higher flicker frequencies than those closer to the fovea. Although this predicts increased behavioural temporal contrast sensitivity in the peripheral visual field, this effect is rarely observed in psychophysical experiments. It is unknown how temporal contrast sensitivity is represented across eccentricity within cortical visual field maps and whether such sensitivities reflect the response properties of retinal cells or … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
36
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
3

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 33 publications
(40 citation statements)
references
References 59 publications
4
36
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This finds support in previous studies in which magnifying stimulus size alone accounted for eccentricity effects in visual search tasks (Carrasco & Frieder, 1997;Chan & Courtney, 1998), temporal contrast sensitivity (Himmelberg & Wade, 2019), and letter recognition (Anstis, 1974;Farrell & Desmarais, 1990;Higgins, Arditi, & Knoblauch, 1996). If these eccentricity-dependent differences in sensitivity can be accounted for by stimulus size alone, it is reasonable to theorize that isoeccentric differences in contrast sensitivity are also rooted in the differences in the spatial scale of cortical representations (i.e.…”
Section: Eccentricity-dependent Decreases In Contrast Sensitivity At supporting
confidence: 82%
“…This finds support in previous studies in which magnifying stimulus size alone accounted for eccentricity effects in visual search tasks (Carrasco & Frieder, 1997;Chan & Courtney, 1998), temporal contrast sensitivity (Himmelberg & Wade, 2019), and letter recognition (Anstis, 1974;Farrell & Desmarais, 1990;Higgins, Arditi, & Knoblauch, 1996). If these eccentricity-dependent differences in sensitivity can be accounted for by stimulus size alone, it is reasonable to theorize that isoeccentric differences in contrast sensitivity are also rooted in the differences in the spatial scale of cortical representations (i.e.…”
Section: Eccentricity-dependent Decreases In Contrast Sensitivity At supporting
confidence: 82%
“…Here we measured lamina-resolved fMRI responses from human participants as they viewed visual stimuli and were required to attend to a specific stimulus feature (orientation). Our stimulus paradigm was designed to elicit concurrent bottom-up and top-down modulations of the stimulus-driven response through orthogonal manipulations of stimulus contrast (bottom-up) and feature-based attention (top-down), both of which are known to influence early visual cortex responses (Boynton et al, 1999; Himmelberg & Wade, 2019; Kamitani & Tong, 2005; Martinez-Trujillo & Treue, 2004; Saenz, Buracas, & Boynton, 2002; Treue & Martinez Trujillo, 1999).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Two-channel models are frequently used to account for different outcomes based on stimulus features, such as motion speed (Bullier & Nowak, 1995;Himmelberg & Wade, 2019;Stone, Dreher, & Leventhal, 1979). For example, a two-channel model was used to characterize the differences in fMRI-measured responses to changes in luminance (Horiguchi, Nakadomari, Misaki, & Wandell, 2009).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%