1982
DOI: 10.3758/bf03197650
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

EEG correlates of posthypnotically controlled degrees of cognitive arousal

Abstract: Experimental control over five degrees of cognitive (as opposed to organismic) arousal has been developed by hypnotic programming techniques. Previously, these posthypnotic manipulations have been applied to the investigation of diverse topics such as visual discrimination, performance on the Stroop test, selective concentration on color vs, form of consonants, and cognitive "reverberation." The present study explored electroencephalographic (EEG) correlates of the five degrees of cognitive arousal in a task r… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...

Citation Types

0
1
0

Year Published

1996
1996
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
2
1

Relationship

0
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 3 publications
(1 citation statement)
references
References 18 publications
0
1
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In terms of its use as a tool, for instance, Reyher (1961Reyher ( ,1962; see also Brickner & Kubie, 1936;Burns & Reyher, 1976; Matthews, Kirsch, & Allen, 1984;Sheehan, 1969;Sommerschield & Reyher, 1973) used a posthypnotic paradigm to examine the clinical relevance of hypnotically induced psychopathology; Blum (1979; Blum & Green, 1978; Blum, Hauenstein, & Graef, 1968;Blum & Nash, 1982; Blum & Wohl, 1971) used a similar paradigm to manipulate cognitive arousal and mood; and Zimbardo, Andersen, and Kabat (1981; see also Van Denburg & Kurtz, 1989) used posthypnotic suggestions to investigate the psychophysiological consequences of unexplained arousal. In terms of intrinsic interest in the phenomenon itself, for instance, Kellogg (1929) and Patten (1930) examined the duration of posthypnotic responding (see also Damaser, 1964; Edwards, 1963;Kihlstrom & Hoyt, 1988; Weitzenhoffer, 1950); Erickson and Erickson (1941;see also McCue, 1992;Reyher & Smyth, 1971) investigated whether a trance state is associated with the execution of posthypnotic suggestions; and Barber (1958Barber ( ,1962; see also Edwards, 1965;Sheehan & Ome, 1968; Weitzenhoffer, 1957) focused on the role of amnesia in posthypnotic behavior.…”
mentioning
confidence: 98%
“…In terms of its use as a tool, for instance, Reyher (1961Reyher ( ,1962; see also Brickner & Kubie, 1936;Burns & Reyher, 1976; Matthews, Kirsch, & Allen, 1984;Sheehan, 1969;Sommerschield & Reyher, 1973) used a posthypnotic paradigm to examine the clinical relevance of hypnotically induced psychopathology; Blum (1979; Blum & Green, 1978; Blum, Hauenstein, & Graef, 1968;Blum & Nash, 1982; Blum & Wohl, 1971) used a similar paradigm to manipulate cognitive arousal and mood; and Zimbardo, Andersen, and Kabat (1981; see also Van Denburg & Kurtz, 1989) used posthypnotic suggestions to investigate the psychophysiological consequences of unexplained arousal. In terms of intrinsic interest in the phenomenon itself, for instance, Kellogg (1929) and Patten (1930) examined the duration of posthypnotic responding (see also Damaser, 1964; Edwards, 1963;Kihlstrom & Hoyt, 1988; Weitzenhoffer, 1950); Erickson and Erickson (1941;see also McCue, 1992;Reyher & Smyth, 1971) investigated whether a trance state is associated with the execution of posthypnotic suggestions; and Barber (1958Barber ( ,1962; see also Edwards, 1965;Sheehan & Ome, 1968; Weitzenhoffer, 1957) focused on the role of amnesia in posthypnotic behavior.…”
mentioning
confidence: 98%