2019
DOI: 10.1016/j.trf.2019.10.004
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Effectiveness of a strategic hazard anticipation training intervention in high risk scenarios

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
8
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8
2

Relationship

3
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 18 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 23 publications
0
8
0
Order By: Relevance
“…As an alternative to an actual cell phone conversation, a mock cell phone task was used in this study, since unscripted conversations are more difficult to control experimentally. Previous studies have shown that this type of secondary task can approximately induce the same amount of cognitive load as a hands-free cell phone conversation ( 25 , 32 ). The mock cell phone task induces cognitive load by requiring participants to listen and respond to a series of sentences, similar to a grammatical reasoning task validated in previous studies ( 33 , 34 ).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 93%
“…As an alternative to an actual cell phone conversation, a mock cell phone task was used in this study, since unscripted conversations are more difficult to control experimentally. Previous studies have shown that this type of secondary task can approximately induce the same amount of cognitive load as a hands-free cell phone conversation ( 25 , 32 ). The mock cell phone task induces cognitive load by requiring participants to listen and respond to a series of sentences, similar to a grammatical reasoning task validated in previous studies ( 33 , 34 ).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 93%
“…Young drivers who have been involved in road crashes may fail to perceive risk in a holistic way [ 27 , 28 , 29 ] and focus on specific hazards rather than the overall level of risk that a certain situation may involve [ 30 ]. They may also be slower at detecting common hazardous situations [ 8 , 31 , 32 ], which may influence risk-taking behaviours through poor understanding of on-road dangers [ 33 ]. Young drivers have been found to be less likely to rate speeding as high risk than older drivers [ 34 , 35 ].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…While some articles focused on the cognitive component, especially to find out how a hands-free phone conversation affects the driver’s performance [ 66 , 71 , 76 , 90 , 91 , 92 , 93 ], others considered two, three, or even four types of distractions. For example, cognitive and manual components were analyzed in [ 94 , 95 , 96 ], cognitive and visual components were presented in [ 25 , 97 ], and cognitive, visual, and manual attributes of the distraction were evaluated in [ 26 , 98 ] or [ 99 ]. As we have seen, only one article considered all four components of distraction: [ 100 ].…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%