Proceedings of the 7th International Conference on Automotive User Interfaces and Interactive Vehicular Applications 2015
DOI: 10.1145/2799250.2799275
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Effects of age and smartphone experience on driver behavior during address entry

Abstract: A Samsung Galaxy S4 and Apple iPhone 5s were compared in a driving simulator where participants performed visual-manual and auditory-vocal address entry tasks. Auditory-vocal tasks were associated with shorter task times, fewer off-road glances, lower workload ratings, and reduced impact on vehicle performance. Primarily nominal differences were found between devices. Older participants had more difficulty performing tasks across both modalities, and difficulties were amplified for visualmanual tasks. A patter… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

0
3
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

1
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 10 publications
0
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…There is no clear consensus on what constitutes an acceptable interaction time for a secondary task. Problematically, the issue is confounded by research suggesting that secondary tasks are often sensitive to whether testing is completed in a static (i.e., not driving) or dynamic (i.e., driving) environment (Young et al, 2005), the age of participants (McWilliams, Reimer, Mehler, Dobres, & Coughlin, 2015), and performance characteristics of the primary or secondary tasks (Tsimhoni, Yoo, & Green, 1999). Because of the visual demands associated with driving, visual secondary tasks generally take longer to complete when performed concurrently with driving.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…There is no clear consensus on what constitutes an acceptable interaction time for a secondary task. Problematically, the issue is confounded by research suggesting that secondary tasks are often sensitive to whether testing is completed in a static (i.e., not driving) or dynamic (i.e., driving) environment (Young et al, 2005), the age of participants (McWilliams, Reimer, Mehler, Dobres, & Coughlin, 2015), and performance characteristics of the primary or secondary tasks (Tsimhoni, Yoo, & Green, 1999). Because of the visual demands associated with driving, visual secondary tasks generally take longer to complete when performed concurrently with driving.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Existing systems were shown to be distracting [14], [15], and hard to use by older adults [16], [17], which shapes a big part of our participants. Old adults tend to have lower visual and motor abilities due to the aging process [18].…”
Section: Literature Reviewmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The question of distraction has been a central one for driving research and driver safety in recent years, particularly with the advent of smartphones. The consensus is that using a smartphone while driving increases operational errors (McWilliams, Reimer, Mehler, & Dobres, 2015;Reimer, Mehler, & Donmez, 2014;Reimer, Mehler, Reagan, Kidd, & Dobres, 2016;Samost et al, 2016;Strayer, Cooper, & Drews, 2004;Strayer & Drews, 2007;Strayer, Drews, & Crouch, 2006;Strayer, Drews, & Johnston, 2003), and this is broadly interpreted as being a result of the driver's need to divide their attention between the phone and the road environment. Other recent changes in the vehicle have similar consequences, in particular the shift from manual switches to touchscreens, which require the driver to look at and attend to them in order to change settings (Chiang, Brooks, & Weir, 2001;Kidd, Dobres, Reagan, Mehler, & Reimer, 2017;Lee, Mehler, Reimer, & Coughlin, 2016;Strayer, Cooper, Turrill, Coleman, & Hopman, 2016;Tsimhoni, Smith, & Green, 2004;Watson & Strayer, 2010).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%