1976
DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.2.2.142
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Effects of orienting task and spacing of repetitions on frequency judgments.

Abstract: Frequency judgments of words repeated 2, 3, and 5 times in a list with 0, 1, 2, 4, 8, or 16 items intervening between repetitions were examined under different instructional sets. In Experiment 1, separate groups of subjects were given one of three types of instructions prior to list presentation: that a frequency judgment test would follow (intentional instructions), that a memory test would follow (nonspecific instructions), or that they were simply required to rate each word on its connotative strength with… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

12
80
1

Year Published

1983
1983
2009
2009

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 77 publications
(93 citation statements)
references
References 17 publications
12
80
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The most straightforward explanation of our findings is a deficient-processing account similar to that proposed in the context of cuedmemory research (Challis, 1993;Rose & Rowe, 1976;Russo et al, 1998). When conceptually driven, explicit tests, such as free recall, are involved, performance is as-spacing effect.…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 61%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…The most straightforward explanation of our findings is a deficient-processing account similar to that proposed in the context of cuedmemory research (Challis, 1993;Rose & Rowe, 1976;Russo et al, 1998). When conceptually driven, explicit tests, such as free recall, are involved, performance is as-spacing effect.…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 61%
“…Our results both support and place limits on the hypothesis that the spacing effect in free recall is the product of be minimized because it is highly primed by the first presentation (Challis, 1993), or because it is recognized as a repetition, and processing is terminated without the need for elaborate semantic processing (Rose, 1980;Rose & Rowe, 1976). Although these hypotheses have been formulated in the context of research on cued-memory tasks involving conceptually driven processes (e.g., frequency judgments of meaningful stimuli), there is no obvious reason why similar processes could not influence other conceptually driven retrieval tasks, such as free recall.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 57%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The ability to judge other details of a word's prior presentation begins about 100 msec later (Gronlund, Edwards, & Ohrt, 1997;Hintzman, 2000;Hintzman & Caulton, 1997;Hintzman & Curran, 1994). Sixth, encoding manipulations such as levels of processing (Fisk & Schneider, 1984;Naveh-Benjamin & Jonides, 1985;Rose & Rowe, 1976;Rowe, 1974), read versus generate (Greene, 1988), and spacing of repetitions (Hintzman, 1969;Hintzman & Block, 1970;Hintzman & Rogers, 1973;Underwood, 1969) have effects on JOF magnitude that parallel their effects on recognition accuracy. Seventh, materials manipulations that produce mirror effects in recognition memory (common vs. rare words, and nouns vs. consonant strings) also produce mirror effects in the discrimination of frequencies (Greene & Thapar, 1994).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It could be argued that the habituation hypothesis is only concerned with processes related to deliberate encoding into memory and, hence, that these RT studies are irrelevant. However, with this restriction, the theory is now unable to account for the spacing effect found in incidental learning (McFarland, Rhodes, & Frey, 1979;Rose, 1980;Rose & Rowe, 1976). A different approach, presented by Jacoby (1978), emphasizes an analogy between memorizing a word and solving a problem.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%