2007
DOI: 10.1002/acp.1426
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Effects of presentation speed of a dynamic visualization on the understanding of a mechanical system

Abstract: In an experimental study, the role of temporal variation of a realistic animation was examined. The animation of a complex mechanical system, a pendulum clock, was presented in a between subject design at normal or fast speed. Presentation speed was found to affect distribution of attention and understanding of the functionality of the clockwork mechanism. Verbal reports in the fast condition contained more statements on the weight, which is a central part of the clocks' mechanism. When giving a written descri… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

3
27
0

Year Published

2009
2009
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

4
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 58 publications
(30 citation statements)
references
References 38 publications
3
27
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Cronbach's alpha was .28 and correlations showed that the item "scale own performance" was negatively correlated with the other items. Therefore, we computed the cognitive load measure from the sum of all items with the exception of "scale own performance", for which the Cronbach's alpha was .58 (this finding is in line with results from Fischer, Lowe, & Schwan, 2008). The cognitive load was comparable across all presentation modes, F < 1 (split screen: M = 33.73, SD = 13.36, vexing image: M = 38.47, SD = 13.22, and overlaid: M = 41.71, SD = 13.59).…”
Section: Cognitive Loadsupporting
confidence: 67%
“…Cronbach's alpha was .28 and correlations showed that the item "scale own performance" was negatively correlated with the other items. Therefore, we computed the cognitive load measure from the sum of all items with the exception of "scale own performance", for which the Cronbach's alpha was .58 (this finding is in line with results from Fischer, Lowe, & Schwan, 2008). The cognitive load was comparable across all presentation modes, F < 1 (split screen: M = 33.73, SD = 13.36, vexing image: M = 38.47, SD = 13.22, and overlaid: M = 41.71, SD = 13.59).…”
Section: Cognitive Loadsupporting
confidence: 67%
“…Though more popular in other science domains, the use of multi-dimensional rating scales for workload such as the NASA-TLX is exceptional within educational science (for examples see, Fischer et al 2008;Gerjets et al 2006;Kester et al 2006b). When cognitive load is measured as one concept, there is no distinction between intrinsic load, extraneous load, and germane load.…”
Section: Overall Problems With Cognitive Load Measuresmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…While the conditions of its instructional effectiveness are still unclear, the factors that influence the processing of animation have been largely identified. Three categories may be distinguished; those a) specific to the learners, such as their prior knowledge level (ChanLin, 1998;Kalyuga, 2008) and visuospatial ability (Hegarty & Sims, 1994;Lowe & Boucheix, 2009;Yang, Andre, & Greenbowe, 2003), b) specific to the instructional material, such as the type of dynamic changes within the animation (Lowe, 2003), its perceptual salience (Lowe & Boucheix, 2009;Schnotz & Lowe, 2003), the presence of accompanying information (Ginns, 2005;Moreno & Mayer, 1999;Tabbers, 2001) or the control over the pace of the animation (Fischer, Lowe, & Schwan, 2007;Mayer & Chandler, 2001), and c) specific to the learning context e e.g., the type of knowledge and the instructional domain Schneider, 2007).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%