2006
DOI: 10.3758/bf03193411
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Effects of repetition and response deadline on item recognition in young and older adults

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
17
0

Year Published

2006
2006
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 17 publications
(21 citation statements)
references
References 47 publications
4
17
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This result obtains with study repetitions (Jacoby, 1999) and increased spacing of repetitions (Benjamin & Craik, 2001), and analogous results are apparent in plurality-reversal recognition (Light et al, 2006), associative recognition (Jones & Jacoby, 2001), and semantic false memory (Benjamin, 2001; Watson, McDermott, & Balota, 2004). …”
Section: Experiments 3: Nonmonotonic False-alarm Rate Functions In Excsupporting
confidence: 52%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…This result obtains with study repetitions (Jacoby, 1999) and increased spacing of repetitions (Benjamin & Craik, 2001), and analogous results are apparent in plurality-reversal recognition (Light et al, 2006), associative recognition (Jones & Jacoby, 2001), and semantic false memory (Benjamin, 2001; Watson, McDermott, & Balota, 2004). …”
Section: Experiments 3: Nonmonotonic False-alarm Rate Functions In Excsupporting
confidence: 52%
“…This result also helps explain why younger subjects under deadlined conditions (Benjamin, 2001; Benjamin & Bjork, 2000; Benjamin & Craik, 2001; Light et al, 2004, 2006) exhibit qualitatively similar patterns to the elderly. It is not because such manipulations selectively affect recollection.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 77%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Enhancing lure familiarity increases rates of false alarms in older but not younger adults (Edmonds et al, 2012; Fandakova et al, 2013a, 2013b; Jacoby, 1999; Jones & Jacoby, 2005; note, however, that age-differences are not always found for familiarity or fluency-based memory illusions; see Thapar & Westerman, 2009). When younger adults are placed under time pressure to reduce their ability to use recollective strategies, they respond similarly to older adults (Jones and Jacoby, 2005; Light et al, 2006). Older adults with poor associative memory or executive functioning are particularly reliant on familiarity when making recognition decisions (Fandakova et al, 2013a; Parkin and Walter, 1992), consistent with a reduced availability of recollective information due to MTL and PFC decline.…”
Section: Mechanisms Of False Memory Formation With Agementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Another possible way to improve older adults' associative memory is to increase the number of presentations for each pair. This method was explored by Light and her colleagues (Light et al, 2004;Light, Chung, Pendergrass, & Van Ocker, 2006) who repeated word pairs within a given study list and gave associative tests consisting of intact pairs, recombined pairs, and pairs of unstudied words. Light et al (2004) found that repetition increased hit rates for intact pairs in both younger and older adults, but it also impeded memory in older adults by increasing their false alarm rate to recombined pairs, which did not occur in younger adults under unconstrained time conditions.…”
Section: Remember/know Judgmentsmentioning
confidence: 99%