2015 6th IEEE International Conference on Cognitive Infocommunications (CogInfoCom) 2015
DOI: 10.1109/coginfocom.2015.7390628
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Effects of text difficulty and readers on predicting reading comprehension from eye movements

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
7
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
3
2
2
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 24 publications
0
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Regression fixations were any fixations that were on a word with an index lower than that of the previous word that was fixated on. These features have been used in similar modeling efforts (Copeland, 2016;Copeland & Gedeon, 2013;Copeland et al, 2014Copeland et al, , 2015Mart ınez-G omez & Aizawa, 2014) and have been empirically linked to factors that influence text comprehension, including mind-wandering (e.g., Reichle et al, 2010;Uzzaman & Joordens, 2011) and text difficulty (Rayner et al, 2006). Although it is not an eye movement feature, we also included page reading time in seconds as it is perhaps the most ubiquitous dependent measure in reading research and has been linked to comprehension (e.g., Mills, Graesser, Risko, & D'Mello, 2017).…”
Section: Eye Movement Featuresmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Regression fixations were any fixations that were on a word with an index lower than that of the previous word that was fixated on. These features have been used in similar modeling efforts (Copeland, 2016;Copeland & Gedeon, 2013;Copeland et al, 2014Copeland et al, , 2015Mart ınez-G omez & Aizawa, 2014) and have been empirically linked to factors that influence text comprehension, including mind-wandering (e.g., Reichle et al, 2010;Uzzaman & Joordens, 2011) and text difficulty (Rayner et al, 2006). Although it is not an eye movement feature, we also included page reading time in seconds as it is perhaps the most ubiquitous dependent measure in reading research and has been linked to comprehension (e.g., Mills, Graesser, Risko, & D'Mello, 2017).…”
Section: Eye Movement Featuresmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, Copeland, Gedeon, and Mendis (2014) had participants read text presented on slides (as in a slide deck). They trained artificial neural networks on several eye movement features computed for each slide (e.g., number of fixations, mean fixation duration, total text fixation duration, number of regressions) to generate predictions of comprehension as assessed by performance on questions presented alongside or immediately after each slide (see also Copeland, 2016; Copeland & Gedeon, 2013; Copeland, Gedeon, & Caldwell, 2015). Similarly, Martínez‐Gómez and Aizawa (2014) achieved above‐chance predictions of binarized (high vs. low) comprehension for short (~450 word) educational texts using a combination of linguistic features (e.g., word length) and eye movement features, with the latter being more discriminative (see also Lou, Liu, Kaakinen, & Li, 2017 for models predicting language skill).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…They used artificial neural networks to predict performance on the comprehension questions from multiple global eye-movement measures (e.g., average fixation duration), with 79-89% accurate classification rate (correct, half-correct, incorrect response). Although this type of method does not allow for a clear links to be established between specific eye-movement measures and comprehension, the results do suggest that eye movements can be used to successfully predict comprehension scores (see also Copeland, Gedeon, & Caldwell, 2016;Copeland & Gedeon, 2013;Martínez-Gómez & Aizawa, 2014). Inhoff, Gregg, and Radach (2018) investigated the predictive relationship between subsets of eye movements and comprehension ability more directly.…”
Section: Predicting Comprehension Accuracy From Eye-movement Behaviourmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The emergence of e-learning platforms has led to the development of more unobtrusive, objective and reliable methods for gathering data, using the logging capabilities afforded by these platforms (Cocea and Weibelzahl, 2011). Different data sources can be instrumented and monitored, including learners' clickstreams (Siemens, 2013), eyes movements (Copeland et al, 2015), participation (Xing et al, 2015), and/or assessments (Fidalgo-Blanco et al, 2015;Snodgrass-Rangel et al, 2015). During the data analysis process, the captured data undergo different transformations, in order to be finally translated into understandable and exploitable human knowledge.…”
Section: Monitoring E-learningmentioning
confidence: 99%