2023
DOI: 10.1101/2023.02.04.527146
|View full text |Cite
Preprint
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Effort cost of harvest affects decisions and movement vigor of marmosets during foraging

Abstract: We prefer to decline effortful rewards, but if the circumstances require it, we will move only slowly to harvest them. Why should economic variables such as reward and effort affect movement vigor? In theory, our decisions and movements both contribute to a measure of fitness in which the objective is to maximize rewards minus efforts, divided by time. To test this idea, we engaged marmosets in a foraging task in which on each trial they decided whether to work by making saccades to visual targets, thus accumu… Show more

Help me understand this report
View published versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2024
2024
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
3

Relationship

1
2

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 3 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 56 publications
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This food was presented via either the left or the right tube for 50-300 consecutive trials, and then switched tubes. Because the amounts were small (0.015-0.02 mL), and the tubes were located at ±90 o with respect to the mouth, harvesting was effortful (22), requiring skillful movements toward a target that was just large enough to accommodate the tongue (4.4 mm diameter tube). As a result, the subjects naturally chose to work for a few consecutive saccade trials (n=6.1±0.02 successful trials per work period), allowing the food to accumulate, then stopped making saccades to targets and initiated their harvest via a bout of licking (n=22.03 ± 0.04 licks per harvest period, Fig.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This food was presented via either the left or the right tube for 50-300 consecutive trials, and then switched tubes. Because the amounts were small (0.015-0.02 mL), and the tubes were located at ±90 o with respect to the mouth, harvesting was effortful (22), requiring skillful movements toward a target that was just large enough to accommodate the tongue (4.4 mm diameter tube). As a result, the subjects naturally chose to work for a few consecutive saccade trials (n=6.1±0.02 successful trials per work period), allowing the food to accumulate, then stopped making saccades to targets and initiated their harvest via a bout of licking (n=22.03 ± 0.04 licks per harvest period, Fig.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…6B). To test if this modulation varied with tongue kinematics, we quantified lick vigor, defined as the peak speed of a given protraction with respect to the speed expected for a lick of the same amplitude (22,26) (Supplementary Figure 5). High vigor licks exhibited greater SS rates at peak protraction speed (Fig.…”
Section: The Population Ss Rates Encoded Parameters Of Tongue Kinematicsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As the motivational state of the subject changed, so did saccade peak velocity ( 35 ). We measured peak velocity as a function of saccade amplitude in each subject and then normalized the data to compute vigor of each saccade ( 36 , 37 ).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Altogether, these data show that the latSC contains a mechanosensorimotor map for touch-guided tongue control that has many similar mechanistic features observed in visually-guided orienting 24,25 . With recent advances examining tongue kinematics in marmosets, it will be possible to test if primate latSC also implements touch-guided control 54 .…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%