2005
DOI: 10.1007/s00359-005-0058-5
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Electric interactions through chirping behavior in the weakly electric fish, Apteronotus leptorhynchus

Abstract: The weakly electric fish Apteronotus leptorhynchus produces wave-like electric organ discharges distinguished by a high degree of regularity. Transient amplitude and frequency modulations ("chirps") can be evoked in males by stimulation with the electric field of a conspecific. During these interactions, the males examined in this study produced six types of chirps, including two novel ones. Stimulation of a test fish with a conspecific at various distances showed that two electrically interacting fish must be… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

13
78
4
1

Year Published

2008
2008
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 81 publications
(96 citation statements)
references
References 32 publications
13
78
4
1
Order By: Relevance
“…After contest resolution, the subordinate continued to produce more and longerlasting offs, and added chirps, probably as a more unambiguous submissive signal (Fig.2). The clear submissive information encoded by chirps in G. omarorum contrasts with previous findings in Apteronotus leptorhynchus (Zupanc et al, 2006;Triefenbach and Zakon, 2008;Hupé and Lewis, 2008), in which chirps were interpreted as threat signals associated to aggression. Brachyhypopomus gauderio produced chirps (either dominants or subordinates) and offs (only subordinates) but they did not seem to follow a predictable temporal pattern .…”
Section: Introductioncontrasting
confidence: 99%
“…After contest resolution, the subordinate continued to produce more and longerlasting offs, and added chirps, probably as a more unambiguous submissive signal (Fig.2). The clear submissive information encoded by chirps in G. omarorum contrasts with previous findings in Apteronotus leptorhynchus (Zupanc et al, 2006;Triefenbach and Zakon, 2008;Hupé and Lewis, 2008), in which chirps were interpreted as threat signals associated to aggression. Brachyhypopomus gauderio produced chirps (either dominants or subordinates) and offs (only subordinates) but they did not seem to follow a predictable temporal pattern .…”
Section: Introductioncontrasting
confidence: 99%
“…When two fish are in close enough proximity so that their electric fields interact (within approximately 1m), the interactions of the electric fields continuously produce amplitude and phase modulations (Heiligenberg, 1991;Tan et al, 2005;Stamper et al, 2010). In Apteronotus, fish can also produce rapid, intermittent transients in their electric field (Zupanc et al, 2006;Dunlap et al, 2010), especially during agonistic encounters . In the ELL, the cancellation of the predictable amplitude modulations that result from the mixing of electric organ discharges (EODs) from nearby fish also induces a concomitant enhancement of the responses to unpredictable chirp signals (Marsat and Maler, 2011).…”
Section: Tail Bending Contributes To Sensory Processingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…olfactory signal) in the stimulus fish. However, in short-term interactions, chirping of the focal fish is quantitatively correlated (Dunlap, 2002) and temporally structured in an 'echo' pattern (Zupanc et al, 2006; with chirping in a stimulus fish. Thus it is likely that pharmacological manipulation of the stimulus fish decreased long-term chirp rates in focal fish by decreasing chirp stimuli from the stimulus fish.…”
Section: Chirping During Dyadic Interactionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Chirp rate is also influenced strongly by stimulus amplitude (Bastian et al, 2001;Dunlap et al, 1998), and, because EOD amplitude attenuates drastically through the water, the distance between fish determines chirp production (Dunlap and Larkins-Ford, 2003;Zupanc et al, 2006): higher stimulus amplitudes and shorter distances between fish elicit higher chirp rates. In both dyadic interactions and chirp chambers, chirp rate habituates relatively quickly after the onset of stimulus, with chirp rates declining to half-maximal values after ~10min of stimulation (Dunlap, 2002;Harvey-Girard et al, 2010).…”
Section: Introduction To Chirping Behavior and Its Neural Controlmentioning
confidence: 99%