Four hens worked under independent multiple concurrent variable-interval schedules with an overlaid aversive stimulus (sound of hens in a poultry shed at lOOdBA) activated by the first peck on a key. The sound remained on until a response was made on the other key. The key that activated the sound in each component was varied over a series of conditions. When the sound was activated by the left (or right) key in one component, it was activated by the right (or left) key in the other component. Bias was examined under a range of different variable-interval schedules, and the applicability of the generalized matching law was examined. It was found that the hens' behavior was biased away from the sound independently of the schedule in effect and that this bias could be quantified using a modified version of the generalized matching law. Behavior during the changeover delays was not affected by the presence of the noise or by changes in reinforcement rate, even though the total response measures were. Insensitivity shown during the delay suggests that behavior after the changeover delay may be more appropriate as a measure of preference (or aversiveness) of stimuli than are overall behavior measures.Key words: concurrent variable-interval schedules, generalized matching law, aversive sounds, behavior during and after changeover delay, key peck, hen Studies of concurrent schedules of reinforcement have most often been concerned with the effects of variations in the rates of reinforcement. In these situations, the relation between behavior and reinforcement rate is well described by the generalized matching law (Davison & McCarthy, 1988). This relation, when the behavior is measured in terms of responses allocated, is expressed in the following equation: log(P1/P2) = a log(rl/r2) + log c, (1) where PI and P2 refer to the responses made on the two alternatives, r1 and r2 are the number of reinforcements obtained on the two alternatives, and a and c are fitted parameters. Baum (1974, 1979) suggested that a measures the sensitivity of the behavior to changes in reinforcement rate. When a is larger than 1.0 (termed overmatching), proportionally more responses have been allocated to the schedules with the richer reinforcement rates than is predicted by an exact match to reinforcement-rate ratios. When a is less than 1.0 (termed undermatching), proportionally more responses have been allocated to the schedules with the leaner reinforcement rates than is predicted by an exact