2020
DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2020.107849
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Electroencephalographic evidence for a reinforcement learning advantage during motor skill acquisition

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
8
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 34 publications
0
8
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Low feedback complexity with a high feedback precision ambiguity (e.g., binary feedback) is assumed to induce higher FRN amplitudes, as compared to a high feedback complexity with low feedback precision ambiguity (e.g., quantitative feedback indicating direction and magnitude of the error). This has already been shown for cognitive tasks (e.g., time estimation task; Cockburn & Holroyd, 2018;Mars et al, 2004), as well as for a simple cursor pointing task (Colino et al, 2020). In experiments in the motor domain, the feedback delay varies between studies.…”
Section: Aim Of the Meta-analysismentioning
confidence: 75%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Low feedback complexity with a high feedback precision ambiguity (e.g., binary feedback) is assumed to induce higher FRN amplitudes, as compared to a high feedback complexity with low feedback precision ambiguity (e.g., quantitative feedback indicating direction and magnitude of the error). This has already been shown for cognitive tasks (e.g., time estimation task; Cockburn & Holroyd, 2018;Mars et al, 2004), as well as for a simple cursor pointing task (Colino et al, 2020). In experiments in the motor domain, the feedback delay varies between studies.…”
Section: Aim Of the Meta-analysismentioning
confidence: 75%
“…If studies had more than one experimental condition (i.e., frequent vs. infrequent/valid vs. invalid/quantitative vs. qualitative feedback), the average of both ERP curves was calculated and used for further analysis. There was one exception to this rule: In the moderator variable analysis, the experimental groups (quantitative vs. qualitative feedback) of Colino et al (2020) were used separately when analyzing the moderator feedback complexity . Data sets with deviating FRN latencies were not excluded because latencies vary a lot in motor studies (Colino et al, 2017; Grand et al, 2017; Maurer et al, 2021; Torrecillos et al, 2014), and it was of special interest to reflect this temporal variability of the FRN in motor tasks.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…On the other hand, in agreement with Graybiel and Grafton [15], after more than a century of work on the motor functions of BG, in the last decades, researchers have focused on the functions in relations to learning habits and acquiring motor skills. Much evidence [15, 16, 17, 18, 19] has been collected and reviewed suggesting a major role of BG in learning behaviors by refining action selection according to an optimization process and shaping skills as a modulator of motor repertoires; and, this learning mechanism supported by striatal circuitry generalize to other domains, including cognitive skills. There is also evidence linking PD to degradation of motor learning [20, 2, 21, 22].…”
Section: Model Descriptionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In spite of that limitation, the fTCD approach used here provided a valid measure for examining 'fast' changes in the neural activation supporting oculomotor inhibition. Further, our group's current work is combining fTCD with electroencephalography (see Colino et al, 2020;Heath et al, 2018) to evaluate source localized inhibitory control for MD saccades.…”
Section: Study Limitationsmentioning
confidence: 99%