2022
DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.992242
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Emotional scene remembering: A combination of disturbing and facilitating effects of emotion?

Abstract: An emotion-induced memory trade-off effect is frequently reported when participants have to memorize complex items that include both neutral and emotional features. This bias corresponds to better remembering of central emotional information accompanied by poor performance related to neutral background information. Although the trade-off effect has been mainly associated with attentional bias toward emotional content, findings suggest that other non-attentional cognitive processes could also be involved. The a… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2

Citation Types

0
2
0

Year Published

2024
2024
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
1
1
1

Relationship

0
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 3 publications
(2 citation statements)
references
References 105 publications
0
2
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Specifically, threat-related arousal can bias individuals to forget contextual information linking peripheral information of an item to its surrounding context in memory (Clewett & Murty, 2019). While, the majority of research has studied threat-related disruptions in associative memory by characterizing item-spatial binding (Bouvarel et al, 2022;Kim et al, 2013;Steinmetz & Kensinger, 2013;Waring et al, 2010) or item-item binding (e.g. Madan et al, 2012;Okada et al, 2011), emerging literature seeks to characterize its role in item-temporal context binding.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Specifically, threat-related arousal can bias individuals to forget contextual information linking peripheral information of an item to its surrounding context in memory (Clewett & Murty, 2019). While, the majority of research has studied threat-related disruptions in associative memory by characterizing item-spatial binding (Bouvarel et al, 2022;Kim et al, 2013;Steinmetz & Kensinger, 2013;Waring et al, 2010) or item-item binding (e.g. Madan et al, 2012;Okada et al, 2011), emerging literature seeks to characterize its role in item-temporal context binding.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In general, observers remember positive scenes (e.g., children blowing bubbles) more quickly and with more detail than neutral and negative scenes (e.g., house on fire) (Melcher, 2010). Compared to neutral images, negative images lead to better memory only when a certain negative object serves as the focus within a complex scene, such that observers' attention is drawn toward this emotionally salient object without being able to fully process the rest of the scene (Bouvarel et al, 2022). In contrast, when a scene contains no focal negative object but gives a negative impression overall, memory performance of such a negative scene does not exceed neutral scenes.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%