2008
DOI: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2008.00056.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Emotions and the Development of Childhood Depression: Bridging the Gap

Abstract: There is a gap between scientific knowledge about typical and atypical emotional development and efforts to identify and serve children's mental health needs. The gap can be bridged with research that integrates clinical perspectives into the study of emotional development. We illustrate this by discussing typical emotional development in early childhood and how it differs from the atypical features of emotion seen among preschool-age children with depression. We suggest new research directions that integrate … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1

Citation Types

4
61
0
5

Year Published

2010
2010
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
6
4

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 83 publications
(70 citation statements)
references
References 89 publications
(102 reference statements)
4
61
0
5
Order By: Relevance
“…Therefore, late middle childhood (i.e., ages 8-12), the developmental period preceding adolescence, may represent an opportune ontogenetic window for studying risk factors for the development of depression, such as ruminative response styles (Hankin et al, 1998). Consistent with a developmental psychopathology framework, an important first step in identifying stress response profiles that may enhance risk for future depressive disorders is to evaluate concurrent associations among stress response and rumination in individuals who are not currently depressed (Cole, Luby, & Sullivan, 2008;Kovacs et al, 2008).…”
Section: Examining Risk Factors For Depressionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Therefore, late middle childhood (i.e., ages 8-12), the developmental period preceding adolescence, may represent an opportune ontogenetic window for studying risk factors for the development of depression, such as ruminative response styles (Hankin et al, 1998). Consistent with a developmental psychopathology framework, an important first step in identifying stress response profiles that may enhance risk for future depressive disorders is to evaluate concurrent associations among stress response and rumination in individuals who are not currently depressed (Cole, Luby, & Sullivan, 2008;Kovacs et al, 2008).…”
Section: Examining Risk Factors For Depressionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Consequently, we expected that children’s performance in “hot” EC tasks would be particularly predictive of those developmental outcomes that also heavily regress on emotional regulation—mostly behavioral problems. The inability to regulate emotion has been strongly implied in the emergence of both internalizing and externalizing psychopathology and mental health (Cole and Deater-Deckard 2009; Cole et al 2008; Eisenberg et al 2004a; Frick and Morris 2004; Nigg 2006; Thompson et al 1995). Krueger et al (1996) found that the inability to delay gratification was associated with externalizing disorders in preadolescents.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Second, and consistent with cognitive theories, emotion theory postulates that negative and positive emotions play a prominent role in depression (Clark, 2005). Higher levels of negative emotion, such as anger, are hypothesized to characterize both depression and anxiety, whereas depression is differentiated relatively more by reduced levels of positive emotion (Clark, 2005; Clark & Watson, 1991) and a predominant emphasis of sadness as a core emotional feature (Cole, Luby, & Sullivan, 2008). Third and related to hierarchical models of emotional disorders, structural models of personality factors predisposing for anxiety and depression (e.g., Brown, Chorpita, & Barlow, 1998; Mineka, Watson, & Clark, 1998) would similarly organize how attentional biases would relate to specific emotional face types.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%