2017
DOI: 10.1016/j.bandc.2017.06.011
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Encoding focus alters diagnostic recollection and event-related potentials (ERPs)

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
10
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 47 publications
0
10
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This positive-going ERP modulation approximately 500-800 ms after the retrieval cue is well established as an index of recollection as opposed to familiarity in recognition tests, being larger when source memory is successful than unsuccessful, as well as when participants report subjective recollection rather than familiarity (Duzel et al, 1997 ; Rugg & Curran, 2007 ; Wilding et al, 1995 ). It also is larger when more information is recollected (Leynes & Mok, 2017 ) or recollected source information has greater precision (Vilberg & Rugg, 2009 ). When recollection is selective, the left parietal effect is larger for targets than non-targets, and non-target activity may be indistinguishable from new (for meta-analysis see Rosburg & Mecklinger, 2017 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This positive-going ERP modulation approximately 500-800 ms after the retrieval cue is well established as an index of recollection as opposed to familiarity in recognition tests, being larger when source memory is successful than unsuccessful, as well as when participants report subjective recollection rather than familiarity (Duzel et al, 1997 ; Rugg & Curran, 2007 ; Wilding et al, 1995 ). It also is larger when more information is recollected (Leynes & Mok, 2017 ) or recollected source information has greater precision (Vilberg & Rugg, 2009 ). When recollection is selective, the left parietal effect is larger for targets than non-targets, and non-target activity may be indistinguishable from new (for meta-analysis see Rosburg & Mecklinger, 2017 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Importantly, it was not possible to contrast ERP responses elicited by correct versus incorrect source attributions, which should be examined in future studies. This would clarify how valence and self-referential encoding influence source memory recognition in accurate (containing source-specifying features) versus erroneous source decisions (characterized by retrieval attempts containing other features that are not sourcespecifying; Cansino et al, 2012;Cansino & Trejo-Morales, 2008;Leynes & Mok, 2017;Mao et al, 2015). Together with fact that the sample was mainly composed by female participants (see Glaser, Mendrek, Germain, Lakis, & Lavoie, 2012 for an example of sex-related memory variations), these factors limit the generalization of the current findings.…”
Section: Limitationsmentioning
confidence: 82%
“…When considering the parietal old/new effect, typically associated with the recollection of information from the study episode including source features (Addante et al, 2012;Allan & Rugg, 1998;Cansino & Trejo-Morales, 2008;MacLeod & Donaldson, 2017;Peters & Daum, 2009;Trott et al, 1999;Wilding & Rugg, 1997;Woroch & Gonsalves, 2010), the expected advantage for words encoded selfreferentially was not observed. Because the amplitude of the parietal old/new effect is sensitive to variations in the amount of recoverable information from the study episode (Leynes et al, 2005;Leynes & Crawford, 2018;Wilding, 2000;Woroch & Gonsalves, 2010), this result suggests that the amount of information retrieved was likely similar across encoding tasks, regardless of whether it was or not source-specifying (i.e., diagnostic; see Leynes & Crawford, 2018;Leynes & Mok, 2017).…”
Section: Emotion and Self-reference Effects During Recognitionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…To address this concern, source tasks can be refined by adding a confidence or RK judgment to assess non-criterial as well as source recollection (Yonelinas & Jacoby, 1996). This approach was adopted in 11 of the 20 included studies, although these studies either analyzed ERPs according to RK responses and source memory responses separately, or collapsed data across high and low confidence levels, for example to obtain sufficient trials for ERP analysis (Addante et al, 2012;Cruse, 2010;Cruse & Wilding, 2009Leynes & Crawford, 2018;Leynes & Mok, 2017;Mao et al, 2015;Mollison & Curran, 2012;Woroch & Gonsalves, 2010). Similar limitations apply to the associative memory paradigm, although there were not enough such studies in the present meta-analysis to formally examine differences from the other two paradigms (2 experiments contributed all 4 ERP effect sizes; Bader et al, 2010).…”
Section: (See Supplementary Fig 1)mentioning
confidence: 99%