2015
DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.01.037
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Enhanced heat discrimination in congenital blindness

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

1
16
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 19 publications
(17 citation statements)
references
References 28 publications
1
16
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In the case of early blindness, functional and structural brain plasticity allow behavioural adaptation to visual deprivation (Kupers & Ptito, 2014). In humans, early blindness results in enhanced pitch discrimination (Gougoux et al., 2004; Hamilton et al., 2004; Wan et al., 2010), better sound localization (Fieger et al., 2006; Lessard et al., 1998; Roder et al., 1999), enhanced tactile acuity (Beaulieu‐Lefebvre et al., 2011; Van Boven et al., 2000; Goldreich & Kanics, 2003; Wong et al., 2011), lower olfactory threshold (Comoglu et al., 2015; Cuevas et al., 2010), enhanced olfactory discrimination and identification (Cuevas et al., 2010; Renier et al., 2013; Rombaux et al., 2010; Smith et al., 1993) and increased pain sensitivity to cutaneous thermal stimuli (Slimani et al, 2014; Slimani et al., 2015). In line with the brain abilities to adapt to sensory deprivation, several studies indicate that behavioural adaptations in the blind are generally supported by functional and structural brain plasticity (Kupers & Ptito, 2014).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the case of early blindness, functional and structural brain plasticity allow behavioural adaptation to visual deprivation (Kupers & Ptito, 2014). In humans, early blindness results in enhanced pitch discrimination (Gougoux et al., 2004; Hamilton et al., 2004; Wan et al., 2010), better sound localization (Fieger et al., 2006; Lessard et al., 1998; Roder et al., 1999), enhanced tactile acuity (Beaulieu‐Lefebvre et al., 2011; Van Boven et al., 2000; Goldreich & Kanics, 2003; Wong et al., 2011), lower olfactory threshold (Comoglu et al., 2015; Cuevas et al., 2010), enhanced olfactory discrimination and identification (Cuevas et al., 2010; Renier et al., 2013; Rombaux et al., 2010; Smith et al., 1993) and increased pain sensitivity to cutaneous thermal stimuli (Slimani et al, 2014; Slimani et al., 2015). In line with the brain abilities to adapt to sensory deprivation, several studies indicate that behavioural adaptations in the blind are generally supported by functional and structural brain plasticity (Kupers & Ptito, 2014).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This enhanced pain perception persisted for several days, indicating that visual deprivation‐induced long‐lasting changes. More recent studies reported that congenitally blind individuals show thermal pain hypersensitivity compared with sighted individuals (Slimani et al., 2013, 2015). In contrast to the earlier study (Zubek et al., 1964), pain hypersensitivity was not observed in individuals with late‐onset blindness (Slimani et al., 2014).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This would lead to qualitatively different ways of processing threatening information and perceiving pain. Accordingly, several studies have assumed that early blind people might display differences in their sensitivity to pain as compared to normally sighted individuals (Slimani, Danti, Ptito, & Kupers, 2014;Slimani, Ptito, & Kupers, 2015). However, further reports suggested that hypersensitivity to pain in early blindness might be restricted to changes in the processing of C-fibre inputs (Slimani, Plaghki, Ptito, & Kupers, 2016) and would be related to differences in anxiety levels and attention between blind and sighted participants (Holten-Rossing, Slimani, Ptito, Danti, & Kupers, 2018).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%