2017
DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.10.004
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Enhancing Generalization of Visuomotor Adaptation by Inducing Use-dependent Learning

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

4
15
1

Year Published

2018
2018
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 17 publications
(20 citation statements)
references
References 60 publications
4
15
1
Order By: Relevance
“…[2, 4, 25] vs. 30-deg. [3, 13, 15, 16] rotation) and whether the perturbation is provided gradually or abruptly (which in turn can influence the size of aftereffects [2, 20, 22]). The awareness of the perturbation can also vary across individuals.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…[2, 4, 25] vs. 30-deg. [3, 13, 15, 16] rotation) and whether the perturbation is provided gradually or abruptly (which in turn can influence the size of aftereffects [2, 20, 22]). The awareness of the perturbation can also vary across individuals.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It was assumed that this condition only involved explicit learning because the subjects were completely aware of the task goal (i.e., to reach toward the imaginary targets based on an instruction provided by the experimenters). In the latter, subjects were asked to reach toward visual targets under a novel visuomotor rotation condition, which is the typical condition employed in many previous studies in which subjects were not informed of the visuomotor perturbation [915]. Though it is possible that this condition may also involve explicit components, it was assumed that it mainly involved implicit learning because the subjects were unaware of the task goal (i.e., adapt to the novel visuomotor rotation while reaching toward visual targets).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Therefore, we predict that M1 excitability for the ECR muscle during the static condition could have been heightened by the difference of proprioceptive and visual information. Sensorimotor adaptation functionally involves updating an efference copy that estimates the sensory consequences of motor commands (Shadmehr and Mussa-Ivaldi, 1994; Lei et al, 2017). A previous study (Witham et al, 2010) suggested that M1 contributes to the modification of the efference copy, and M1 excitability changes induced by sensorimotor tasks generally take the form of increased excitability (Rosenkranz et al, 2007; Smyth et al, 2010).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Second, that these mechanisms might generalize across contexts differently. There is an ongoing debate regarding the first assumption [31, 37, 39, 40], however, it has been suggested that generalization of model-free learning based on reinforcement signals contributes only locally and to a limited extent to generalization [41, 42].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%