Entrenchment and the Psychology of Language Learning: How We Reorganize and Adapt Linguistic Knowledge. 2017
DOI: 10.1037/15969-007
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Entrenchment from a psycholinguistic and neurolinguistic perspective.

Abstract: Across the various entrenchment definitions given in the cognitive linguistics literature, it is possible to identify the following recurring key ingredients: high frequency of use, great ease of processing, great strength of representation, high fluency of composition, and chunk status. Although the term entrenchment itself has little currency outside of usage-based cognitive linguistics, several strands of neurolinguistic and psycholinguistic research have investigated frequency effects on language processin… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
11
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
3
1

Relationship

2
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 49 publications
(11 citation statements)
references
References 35 publications
0
11
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Ferretti et al, 2018). This means that usage-based and emergentist researchers investigating factors such as the biological properties of the language-ready brain (e.g., Arbib, 2012, 2015), the neurological foundations of entrenchment (Schmid, 2015; Blumenthal-Dramé, 2016), the neurological foundations of semantic simulation (Bergen, 2012), or the neurological foundations of constructions (Pulvermüller et al, 2013; Goldberg, 2019), are doing biolinguistics as well. In this paper, however, our interest in convergences and divergences is somewhat more specific.…”
Section: The Frameworkmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Ferretti et al, 2018). This means that usage-based and emergentist researchers investigating factors such as the biological properties of the language-ready brain (e.g., Arbib, 2012, 2015), the neurological foundations of entrenchment (Schmid, 2015; Blumenthal-Dramé, 2016), the neurological foundations of semantic simulation (Bergen, 2012), or the neurological foundations of constructions (Pulvermüller et al, 2013; Goldberg, 2019), are doing biolinguistics as well. In this paper, however, our interest in convergences and divergences is somewhat more specific.…”
Section: The Frameworkmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Rather than putting together that’s and right or that’s and fine compositionally by means of syntactic operations, speakers who routinely use these patterns probably have them available as ready-made chunks or prefabs in their mental lexicons ( Gobet et al, 2001 ; Ellis 2017 ). However, it is unclear how many repetitions are required to create such a chunk in the mental lexicon, and also, from a methodological point of view, how many attestations would be required as evidence for the existence of such a chunk ( Blumenthal-Dramé 2012 ; Blumenthal-Dramé 2017 ). Therefore, following the arguments put forward by Schmid (2020) , we argue that the chunk-like processing and representation of sequences is best accounted for in terms of particularly strong syntagmatic associations giving rise to a very high sequential predictability.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Further, language processing changes across the lifespan as experience accumulates (and the brain changes) (Brysbaert et al, 2016;Hanulíková et al, 2020). Frequency effects also reflect the impact of experience on language processing; even structurally identical multi-morphemic sequences are processed differently depending on their usage frequency, with higher-frequency sequences (like government or I don't know) eliciting greater processing ease and chunked access relative to rarer ones (like amazement or You don't swim) (Blumenthal-Dramé, 2016a;Blumenthal-Dramé et al, 2017;Carrol and Conklin, 2019).…”
Section: Experience-based Individual Differencesmentioning
confidence: 99%